Autonoetic consciousness

{{Short description|Human ability for introspection}}

Autonoetic consciousness is the human ability to mentally place oneself in the past and future (i.e. mental time travel) or in counterfactual situations (i.e. alternative outcomes), and to thus be able to examine one's own thoughts.

One's sense of self affects their behavior, in the present, past and future. It relates to how one reflects on their own past behavior, how they feel about it, and this in turn determines if they do it again.Baddeley, A., Eysenck, M. W., & Anderson, M. C., Memory (London & New York: Psychology Press), [https://books.google.com/books?id=xxRWBQAAQBAJ&pg=PT308 pp. 308–309].{{rp|308–309}}

It is episodic memory that deals with self-awareness, memories of the self and inward thoughts that may be projected onto future actions of an individual.{{sfn|Baddeley|Eysenck|Anderson|2009}}{{page needed|date=May 2011}} It was "proposed by Endel Tulving for self-awareness, allowing the rememberer to reflect on the contents of episodic memory".{{sfn|Baddeley|Eysenck|Anderson|2015|p=308}} Moreover, autonoetic consciousness involves behaviors such as mental time travel,{{cite journal |last1=Schacter |first1=D. L. |last2=Addis |first2=D. R. |last3=Buckner |first3=R. L. |year=2007 |title=Remembering the Past to Imagine the Future: The Prospective Brain |journal=Nature Reviews Neuroscience |volume=8 |issue=9 |pages=657–661 |doi=10.1038/nrn2213|pmid=17700624 |s2cid=10376207 }} Cited in {{harvnb|Hills|Butterfill|2015|p=369}}.{{cite journal |last1=Suddendorf |first1=T. |last2=Corballis |first2=M. C. |year=2007 |title=The Evolution of Foresight: What Is Mental Time Travel, and Is It Unique to Humans? |journal=Behavioral and Brain Sciences |volume=30 |issue=3 |pages=299–313 |doi=10.1017/s0140525x07001975|pmid=17963565 |url=https://espace.library.uq.edu.au/view/UQ:135952/Suddendorf-04122006.pdf }} Cited in {{harvnb|Hills|Butterfill|2015|p=369}}. self-projection,{{cite journal |last1=Buckner |first1=R. L. |last2=Carroll |first2=D. C. |year=2007 |title=Self-Projection and the Brain |journal=Trends in Cognitive Sciences |volume=11 |issue=2 |pages=49–57 |doi=10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.004|pmid=17188554 |s2cid=12791119 }} Cited in {{harvnb|Hills|Butterfill|2015|p=369}}. and episodic future thinking,{{cite journal |last1=Atance |first1=C. M. |last2=O'Neill |first2=D. K. |year=2001 |title=Episodic Future Thinking |journal=Trends in Cognitive Sciences |volume=5 |issue=12 |pages=533–539 |doi=10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01804-0|pmid=11728911 |s2cid=21995980 }} Cited in {{harvnb|Hills|Butterfill|2015|p=369}}. all of which have often been proposed as exclusively human capacities.{{sfn|Hills|Butterfill|2015|p=369}}

The self

Autonoetic consciousness is important in our formation of our "self" identity. What we have done in the past becomes a part of our "self" and the ability to reflect on this influences our behavior in the now.

In psychology, the self is often used for that set of attributes that a person attaches to themself most firmly, the attributes that the person finds it difficult or impossible to imagine themselves without.{{citation needed|date=August 2019}} Identity is also used to describe this.{{citation needed|date=September 2019}}

In philosophy, the self is the agent, the knower and the ultimate locus of personal identity.{{citation needed|date=September 2019}} This self, the identity of which is at the bottom of every action, and involved in every bit of knowledge, is the self philosophers worry about.{{citation needed|date=September 2019}} Nevertheless, care of the self is of utmost importance in the bios-logos relationship.Foucault, Parrhesia, 2007.{{full citation needed|date=March 2018}}

The parietal cortex

The parietal cortex is strongly involved in autonoetic consciousness. Damage to areas of the parietal cortex can lead to different functioning errors, including changes in personality.

Lesions in the right parietal lobe influence personality, and this could be because the parietal lobe has to do with our sense of self. Our sense of self is strongly reflected in our personality.

Some common tests for parietal lobe function are: Kimura Box Test (apraxia) and the Two-Point Discrimination Test (somatosensory).{{cite web |title=Parietal Lobes |publisher=Centre for Neuro Skills |work=TBI Resource Guide |url=http://www.neuroskills.com/tbi/bparieta.shtml |access-date=2011-02-24 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20101126213303/http://neuroskills.com/tbi/bparieta.shtml |archive-date=2010-11-26 |url-status=dead }}{{Unreliable source?|date=May 2011}}

Episodic memory and the self

For a coherent and meaningful life, conscious self-representation is mandatory.{{sfn|Lou|Luber|Crupain|Keenan|2004|p=6827}} Autonoetic consciousness is thought to emerge by retrieval of memory of personally experienced events (episodic memory).{{sfn|Lou|Luber|Crupain|Keenan|2004|p=6827}} Without the ability to reflect on our past experiences, we would be stuck in a state of constant awakening, without a past and therefore unable to prepare for the future.

Episodic memory is the memory we have for our past experiences, which influence our now, and our future. This is different from procedural memory, which is our memory for how to do things. Episodic memories influence our thinking about ourselves, good and bad.

Autobiographical memories can be retrieved from either the first person perspective, in which individuals see the event through their own eyes, or from the third person perspective, in which individuals see themselves and the event from the perspective of an external observer.{{sfn|Sutin|Robins|2008|p=1386}}

A growing body of research suggests that the visual perspective from which a memory is retrieved has important implications for a person's thoughts, feelings, and goals, and is integrally related to a host of self- evaluative processes.{{sfn|Sutin|Robins|2008|p=1386}}

Social anxiety disorder

Social anxiety disorder (SAD) is an example of how bad experiences can also lead to our behaviors. It demonstrates how our thoughts influence our feelings about ourselves and therefore our actions in society around us. It has to do with a person's self-esteem, fear of failure, shame, fear of offending, and fear of strangers.

Cognitive models of social anxiety disorder believe the social self is a key psychological mechanism that maintains fear of negative evaluation in social and performance situations.{{sfn|Anderson|Goldin|Kurita|Gross|2008}}{{page needed|date=March 2018}} Consequently, a distorted self-view is evident when recalling painful autobiographical social memories, as reflected in linguistic expression, negative self-beliefs, and emotion and avoidance.{{sfn|Anderson|Goldin|Kurita|Gross|2008}}{{page needed|date=March 2018}}

To test this hypothesis, 42 adults diagnosed with SAD and 27 non-psychiatric healthy controls composed autobiographical narratives of distinct social anxiety related situations, generated negative self-beliefs, and provided emotion and avoidance ratings.{{sfn|Anderson|Goldin|Kurita|Gross|2008}}{{page needed|date=March 2018}}

Although narratives were matched for initial emotional intensity and present vividness, linguistic analyses demonstrated that, compared to the control group, the SAD group employed more self-referential, anxiety, and sensory words, and made fewer references to other people.{{sfn|Anderson|Goldin|Kurita|Gross|2008}}{{page needed|date=March 2018}} Social anxiety symptom severity, however, was associated with greater self-referential negative self-beliefs (NSB) in SAD only.{{sfn|Anderson|Goldin|Kurita|Gross|2008}}{{page needed|date=March 2018}}

SAD reported greater current self-conscious emotions when recalling autobiographical social situations, and greater active avoidance of similar situations than did the control group.{{sfn|Anderson|Goldin|Kurita|Gross|2008}}{{page needed|date=March 2018}} Autobiographical memory of social situations in SAD may influence current and future thinking, emotion, and behavioral avoidance.{{sfn|Anderson|Goldin|Kurita|Gross|2008}}{{page needed|date=March 2018}}

See also

References

= Footnotes =

{{reflist|22em}}

= Bibliography =

{{refbegin|35em|indent=yes}}

  • {{cite journal |pages=1119–1125 |doi=10.1016/j.brat.2008.07.001 |title=Self-Representation in Social Anxiety Disorder: Linguistic Analysis of Autobiographical Narratives |year=2008 |last1=Anderson |first1=Barrett |last2=Goldin |first2=Philippe R. |last3=Kurita |first3=Keiko |last4=Gross |first4=James J. |journal=Behaviour Research and Therapy |volume=46 |issue=10 |pmid=18722589 |pmc=2630512 }}
  • {{cite book |last1=Baddeley |first1=Alan |last2=Eysenck |first2=Michael W. |last3=Anderson |first3=Michael C. |year=2009 |title=Memory |location=New York |publisher=Psychology Press |isbn=978-1-84872-000-8 }}
  • {{cite book |last1=Baddeley |first1=Alan |last2=Eysenck |first2=Michael W. |last3=Anderson |first3=Michael C. |author1-mask={{long dash}} |author2-mask= |author3-mask= |year=2015 |title=Memory |edition=2nd |location=New York |publisher=Psychology Press |isbn=978-1-84872-183-8 }}
  • {{cite journal |pages=5973–5978 |doi=10.1073/pnas.94.11.5973 |title=Event-Related Brain Potential Correlates of Two States of Conscious Awareness in Memory |year=1997 |last1=Duzel |first1=Emrah |journal=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences |volume=94 |issue=11 |pmid=9159185 |last2=Yonelinas |first2=Andrew P. |last3=Mangun |first3=George R. |last4=Heinze |first4=Hans-Jochen |last5=Tulving |first5=Endel |pmc=20891 |bibcode=1997PNAS...94.5973D |doi-access=free }}
  • {{cite journal |last1=Hills |first1=Thomas T. |last2=Butterfill |first2=Stephen |year=2015 |title=From Foraging to Autonoetic Consciousness: The Primal Self as a Consequence of Embodied Prospective Foraging |url=http://www.butterfill.com/pdf/primal_self.pdf |journal=Current Zoology |volume=61 |issue=2 |pages=368–381 |access-date=17 March 2018 |doi=10.1093/czoolo/61.2.368|doi-access=free }}
  • {{cite journal |pages=6827–6832 |doi=10.1073/pnas.0400049101 |doi-access=free |title=Parietal Cortex and Representation of the Mental Self |year=2004 |last1=Lou |first1=Hans C. |journal=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences |volume=101 |issue=17 |pmid=15096584 |last2=Luber |first2=Bruce |last3=Crupain |first3=Michael |last4=Keenan |first4=Julian P. |last5=Nowak |first5=Markus |last6=Kjaer |first6=Troels W. |last7=Sackeim |first7=Harold A. |last8=Lisanby |first8=Sarah H. |authorlink7=Harold A. Sackeim |authorlink8=Sarah Lisanby |pmc=404216 |bibcode=2004PNAS..101.6827L }}
  • {{cite journal |pages=672–677 |doi=10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01957.x |title=Self-Reference During Explicit Memory Retrieval: An Event-Related Potential Analysis |year=2007 |last1=Magno |first1=Elena |last2=Allan |first2=Kevin |journal=Psychological Science |volume=18 |issue=8 |pmid=17680935 |s2cid=35603021 }}
  • {{cite journal |pages=1386–1397 |doi=10.1016/j.concog.2008.09.001 |title=When the 'I' Looks at the 'Me': Autobiographical Memory, Visual Perspective, and the Self |year=2008 |last1=Sutin |first1=Angelina R. |last2=Robins |first2=Richard W. |journal=Consciousness and Cognition |volume=17 |issue=4 |pmid=18848783 |pmc=2733231 }}

{{refend}}

Further reading

{{refbegin|35em|indent=yes}}

  • {{cite journal |pages=1351–1361 |doi=10.1098/rstb.2001.0955 |title=Episodic Memory and Autonoetic Consciousness: A First-Person Approach |year=2001 |last1=Gardiner |first1=J. M. |journal=Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences |volume=356 |issue=1413 |pmid=11571027 |pmc=1088519 }}
  • {{cite journal |pages=192–194 |doi=10.1093/scan/nsn031 |title=A Neural Signature of the Current Self |year=2008 |last1=Libby |first1=Lisa K. |journal=Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience |volume=3 |issue=3 |pmid=19015110 |pmc=2566766 }}

{{refend}}

Category:Consciousness

Category:Self