Behavioral enrichment

{{Short description|Techniques for stimulating captive animals}}

{{distinguish|Environmental enrichment}}

Image:Asian elephant enrichment.jpg in a zoo manipulating a suspended ball provided as environmental enrichment]]

Behavioral enrichment is an animal husbandry principle that seeks to enhance the quality of captive animal care by identifying and providing the environmental stimuli necessary for optimal psychological and physiological well-being.Shepherdson, D.J. (1998) “Tracing the path of environmental enrichment in zoos” in Shepherdson, D.J., Mellen, J.D. and Hutchins, M. (1998) Second Nature – Environmental Enrichment for Captive Animals, 1st Edition, Smithsonian Chupapo stitution Press, London, UK, pp. 1–12. Enrichment can either be active or passive, depending on whether it requires direct contact between the animal and the enrichment. A variety of enrichment techniques are used to create desired outcomes similar to an animal's individual and species' history. Each of the techniques used is intended to stimulate the animal's senses similarly to how they would be activated in the wild. Provided enrichment may be seen in the form of auditory, olfactory, habitat factors, food, research projects, training, and objects.{{Cite web|url=https://nationalzoo.si.edu/SCBI/AnimalEnrichment/|title=Animal Enrichment - National Zoo|website=nationalzoo.si.edu|access-date=2016-03-29|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20160401200044/http://nationalzoo.si.edu/SCBI/AnimalEnrichment/|archive-date=2016-04-01|url-status=dead}}

Purpose

Environmental enrichment can improve the overall welfare of animals in captivity and create a habitat similar to what they would experience in their wild environment. It aims to maintain an animal's physical and psychological health by increasing the range or number of species-specific behaviors, increasing positive interaction with the captive environment, preventing or reducing the frequency of abnormal behaviors, such as stereotypies, and increasing the individual's ability to cope with the challenges of captivity.{{Cite web|url=https://www.stlzoo.org/animals/enrichmenttraining/animalenrichment/|title=Animal Enrichment :: Saint Louis Zoo|website=www.stlzoo.org |access-date=2018-04-11}} Stereotypies are seen in captive animals due to stress and boredom. This includes pacing, self-harm, over-grooming, head-weaving, etc.{{Cite web|url=https://awionline.org/content/towards-understanding-stereotypic-behaviour-laboratory-macaques|title=Towards an Understanding of Stereotypic Behaviour in Laboratory Macaques|website=awionline.org|access-date=2019-02-27}}

Environmental enrichment can be offered to any animal in captivity, including:

  • Animals in zoos and related facilities{{cite journal | vauthors = Maple TL | title = Toward a science of welfare for animals in the zoo | journal = Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science | volume = 10 | issue = 1 | pages = 63–70 | year = 2007 | pmid = 17484680 | doi = 10.1080/10888700701277659 | citeseerx = 10.1.1.560.8948 | s2cid = 32358898 }}
  • Animals in sanctuaries{{cite web | url = http://www.2ndchance.info/Enrichment.htm | title = Synopsis of the Environmental Enrichment Program of 2nd Chance Sanctuary | author = Ron Hines, D.V.M. | date = 2006-04-24 | access-date = 2006-06-11 | archive-url= https://web.archive.org/web/20060610021332/http://2ndchance.info/Enrichment.htm| archive-date= 10 June 2006 | url-status= live}}
  • Animals in shelters and adoption centers{{cite journal | vauthors = Bowman A, Dowell FJ, Evans NP | title = The effect of different genres of music on the stress levels of kennelled dogs | journal = Physiology & Behavior | volume = 171 | pages = 207–215 | date = March 2017 | pmid = 28093218 | doi = 10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.01.024 | s2cid = 207377805 | url = http://eprints.gla.ac.uk/135846/1/135846.pdf }}
  • Animals used for research{{cite web|url=http://www.nc3rs.org.uk/downloaddoc.asp?id=603&page=759&skin=0|title=Validating refinements to laboratory housing: asking the animals.|author=Sherwin, C.M.|year=2007|access-date=April 9, 2013|archive-date=January 21, 2012|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20120121045501/http://www.nc3rs.org.uk/downloaddoc.asp?id=603&page=759&skin=0|url-status=dead}}
  • Animals used for companionship, e.g. dogs,{{cite book | vauthors = Hubrecht R | date = 1995 | chapter = Dogs and dog housing | veditors = Smith CP, Taylor V | title = Environmental Enrichment Information Resources for Laboratory Animals | publisher = Universities Federation for Animal Welfare (UFAW) | location = Potters Bar, Herts. | pages = 49–62 | chapter-url = http://www.nal.usda.gov/awic/pubs/enrich/dogs.htm | archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20020404062237/http://www.nal.usda.gov/awic/pubs/enrich/dogs.htm | url-status = dead | archive-date = 4 April 2002 }} cats, rabbits, etc.

Environmental enrichment can be beneficial to a wide range of vertebrates and invertebrates such as land mammals, marine mammals, and amphibians.{{Cite web|url=http://www.ammpa.org/faqs.html#5|title=Alliance of Marine Mammal Parks and Aquariums|website=www.ammpa.org|access-date=2016-03-29|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20160329153357/http://www.ammpa.org/faqs.html#5|archive-date=2016-03-29|url-status=dead}} In the United States, specific regulations (Animal Welfare Act of 1966) must be followed for enrichment plans in order to guarantee, regulate, and provide appropriate living environments and stimulation for animals in captivity.{{cite journal | vauthors = Kulpa-Eddy JA, Taylor S, Adams KM | title = USDA perspective on environmental enrichment for animals | journal = ILAR Journal | volume = 46 | issue = 2 | pages = 83–94 | date = 2005-01-01 | pmid = 15775018 | doi = 10.1093/ilar.46.2.83 | doi-access = free }} Moreover, the Association of Zoos and Aquariums (also known as the AZA), requires that animal husbandry and welfare be a main concern for those caring for animals in captivity.

Passive enrichment

Passive enrichment provides sensory stimulation but no direct contact or control. This type of enrichment is commonly used for its potential to benefit several animals simultaneously as well as requiring limited direct animal contact.{{cite journal | vauthors = Meade TM, Hutchinson E, Krall C, Watson J | title = Use of an aquarium as a novel enrichment item for singly housed rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) | journal = Journal of the American Association for Laboratory Animal Science | volume = 53 | issue = 5 | pages = 472–7 | date = September 2014 | pmid = 25255069 | pmc = 4181688 }}

= Visual enrichment =

Visual enrichment is typically provided by changing the layout of an animal's holding area. The type of visual enrichment can vary, from something as simple as adding pictures on walls to videotapes and television. Visual enrichment such as television can especially benefit animals housed in single cages.{{cite journal | vauthors = Lutz CK, Novak MA | title = Environmental enrichment for nonhuman primates: theory and application | journal = ILAR Journal | volume = 46 | issue = 2 | pages = 178–91 | date = 2005 | pmid = 15775027 | doi = 10.1093/ilar.46.2.178 | doi-access = free }}

Mirrors are also a potential form of enrichment, specifically for animals that display an understanding of self-recognition, such as non-human primates. In addition to using mirrors to reflect the animal's own image, mirrors can also be angled so the animal is able to see normally out-of-sight areas of the holding area.

Enclosures in modern zoos are often designed to facilitate environmental enrichment. For example, the Denver Zoo's exhibit Predator Ridge allows different African carnivores to be rotated among several enclosures, providing the animals with a differently sized environment.

= Auditory enrichment =

In the wild, animals are exposed to a variety of sounds that they normally do not encounter in captivity. Auditory enrichment can be used to mimic the animal's natural habitat. Types of nature-based auditory enrichment include rain forest sounds and con-specific vocalizations.

The most common form of auditory enrichment is music, whose principal stems primarily from its benefit to humans. The benefits of classical music have been widely studied in animals, from sows{{cite journal|last1=Silva|first1=Flávia R. S.|last2=Miranda|first2=Késia O. da S.|last3=Piedade|first3=Sônia M. de S.|last4=Salgado|first4=Douglas D'Alessandro | name-list-style = vanc |title=Effect of Auditory Enrichment (Music) in Pregnant Sows Welfare|journal=Engenharia Agrícola|date=April 2017|volume=37|issue=2|pages=215–225|doi=10.1590/1809-4430-eng.agric.v37n2p215-225/2017|doi-access=free|hdl=11449/169612|hdl-access=free}} to non-human primates.{{cite journal | vauthors = Wallace EK, Altschul D, Körfer K, Benti B, Kaeser A, Lambeth S, Waller BM, Slocombe KE | title = Is music enriching for group-housed captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)? | journal = PLOS ONE | volume = 12 | issue = 3 | pages = e0172672 | date = 2017-03-29 | pmid = 28355212 | pmc = 5371285 | doi = 10.1371/journal.pone.0172672 | bibcode = 2017PLoSO..1272672W | doi-access = free }} Studies have also looked at various other genres, such as pop and rock, but their ability to provide effective enrichment remains inconclusive.{{cite journal | vauthors = Bowman A, Dowell FJ, Evans NP | title = The effect of different genres of music on the stress levels of kennelled dogs | journal = Physiology & Behavior | volume = 171 | pages = 207–215 | date = March 2017 | pmid = 28093218 | doi = 10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.01.024 | s2cid = 207377805 | url = http://eprints.gla.ac.uk/135846/1/135846.pdf }}{{Cite journal |title= Music as enrichment for Sumatran orangutans (Pongo abelii) | journal = Journal of Zoo and Aquarium Research |volume= 4 |issue= 3 |doi= 10.19227/jzar.v4i3.231 |year= 2016 |last1= Ritvo |first1= Sarah E. |last2= MacDonald |first2= Suzanne E. }} Most types of music that are selected for enrichment are based on human preferences, causing anthropomorphic biases that may not translate to other animals. Therefore, music that is specifically attuned to the animal's auditory senses could be beneficial.{{cite journal | vauthors = Kędzierski W, Janczarek I, Stachurska A, Wilk I | title=Massage or music meant to be relaxing, result in lowering salivary cortisol concentration in race horses|journal=Pferdeheilkunde Equine Medicine|volume=33|issue=2|pages=146–151|doi=10.21836/pem20170206| year=2017| doi-broken-date=2024-11-18| s2cid=57651310|doi-access=free}} Species-specific sounds require further research to find what pitch, frequency, and range is most suitable for the animal.

Active enrichment

File:Behavioral enrichment - Feeding.JPGActive enrichment often requires the animal to perform some sort of physical activity as well as direct interaction with the enrichment object. Active enrichment items can temporarily reduce stereotypic behaviors as their beneficial effects are usually limited to the short periods of active use.File:Behavioral enrichment - Sensory.JPG]]

= Food-based enrichment =

Food-based enrichment is meant to mimic what a captive animal would do in the wild for food. This is extremely important because in the wild, animals are adapted to work hard for what they eat. A lot of time and energy is spent finding food, which is why this tactic is used to make it more challenging for the animal rather than just feeding it simple food.{{Cite journal|last=Orangutan Project|date=2015|title=Orangutan Project Enrichment|url=https://projectorangutan.com/wp-content/uploads/2015/01/Enrichment-catalogue-1-Structural.pdf}} Feeding enrichment techniques causes the animal to indulge in natural, active behaviors that allow for more stimulation and prevents boredom. This form of enrichment forms active behaviors that can also help with not only a captive animal's mental health, but the animal's physical health.

For example, food can be hidden and spread across an enclosure making the animal actively search for it. Other common manipulable tactile objects include rubber toys stuffed with treats. Instead of providing the food directly, foraging devices are useful in increasing the amount of searching and foraging of food, comparable to the amount of time they would spend in the wild. Most food-based enrichment occurs in the context of searching for food, such as cracking open a nut or digging holes in tree trunks for worms.{{cn|date=October 2023}}

= Structural enrichment =

File:Hose2Habitat Oakland Zoo (48431016721).jpgs at the Oakland Zoo]]

Structural Enrichment is when objects are added to an enclosure to mimic an animal's natural habitat. These objects can be switched out occasionally or kept permanently.{{Cite journal|last=Orangutan Project|date=2015|title=Orangutan Project Enrichment|url=https://projectorangutan.com/wp-content/uploads/2015/01/Enrichment-catalogue-1-Structural.pdf}} The environment of captive animals should be switched frequently since their environment in the wild would bring on new objects and exploration. Research into what constitutes the most beneficial and appropriate forms of enrichment must be used when considering the provision of enrichment options, especially for species where natural-like settings may be difficult to achieve.{{cite journal|last1=Jones|first1=N.A.R.|last2=Webster|first2=M.|last3=Salvanes|first3=A.G.V.|year=2021|title=Physical enrichment research for captive fish: Time to focus on the DETAILS |url=https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/full/10.1111/jfb.14773|journal=Journal of Fish Biology|volume=99|issue=3|pages=704–725|doi=10.1111/jfb.14773|pmid=33942889|bibcode=2021JFBio..99..704J |hdl=10023/23362 |s2cid=233719781|hdl-access=free}} The animal should never become too familiar with their environment because that can cause boredom, no stimulation or stereotypical behavior. Examples of this could be swings or climbing structures. Stones have also been shown to encourage exploratory behavior in Japanese macaques. Interaction with the stones exhibited behaviors such as gathering, rolling in hands, rubbing, and carrying.

Other common forms include cardboard, forage, and even the texture of the food (i.e. hard, smooth, cold, warm).{{Cite web|url=http://www.arkanimals.com/ark/e_enrichment_101.html#TACTILE|title=Enrichment 101: Environment, Enrichment, & Education|website=www.arkanimals.com|access-date=2018-03-28}}

= Olfactory enrichment =

Olfactory enrichment can stimulate naturalistic behavior, enhance exploration, and reduce inactive behaviors.{{Cite book|title=Chemical Signals in Vertebrates 11|last1=Clark|first1=Fay|last2=King|first2=Andrew J. | name-list-style = vanc |date=2008|publisher=Springer, New York, NY|isbn=9780387739441|pages=391–398|doi=10.1007/978-0-387-73945-8_37}} Olfactory enrichment can be utilized by itself, paired with novel toys, or paired with food-based enrichment. This type of enrichment is most commonly used with species that commonly utilize their olfactory senses in the wild. Although highly beneficial, it is important for researchers to analyze the long-term effects of certain odors on captive animals. Odors can be scattered on a novel toy such as a ball or semi-randomly throughout an enclosure. Various forms of odors can include catnip, odor of conspecific, perfume, feces of a prey species, or spices.

= Cognitive enrichment =

Cognitive enrichment is defined as, improving animal welfare by providing opportunities for captive animals to use cognitive skills for problem solving and providing limited control over some aspects of its environment.{{Cite journal |last=Clark |first=Fay E. |date=November 2011 |title=Great ape cognition and captive care: Can cognitive challenges enhance well-being? |journal=Applied Animal Behaviour Science |language=en |volume=135 |issue=1–2 |pages=1–12 |doi=10.1016/j.applanim.2011.10.010|url=http://researchonline.rvc.ac.uk/id/eprint/5589/ |url-access=subscription }} In the wild, animals deal with ecological challenges in order to acquire the resources, such as food and shelter, that they require to survive.{{Cite journal|last1=Meehan|first1=Cheryl L.|last2=Mench|first2=Joy A.|date=February 2007|title=The challenge of challenge: Can problem solving opportunities enhance animal welfare?|journal=Applied Animal Behaviour Science|language=en|volume=102|issue=3–4|pages=246–261|doi=10.1016/j.applanim.2006.05.031}} These challenges arise from interactions with other animals, or through changes to their environment that require the individuals to exercise their cognitive ability and to improve their behavioral strategies. Therefore, these challenges act as an important problem-solving element in the animals' day-to-day lives, and in-turn, increases their overall fitness. The animal anticipates positive benefits from a challenging situation{{Cite journal |last1=Boissy |first1=Alain |last2=Manteuffel |first2=Gerhard |last3=Jensen |first3=Margit Bak |last4=Moe |first4=Randi Oppermann |last5=Spruijt |first5=Berry |last6=Keeling |first6=Linda J. |last7=Winckler |first7=Christoph |last8=Forkman |first8=Björn |last9=Dimitrov |first9=Ivan |last10=Langbein |first10=Jan |last11=Bakken |first11=Morten |date=October 2007 |title=Assessment of positive emotions in animals to improve their welfare |journal=Physiology & Behavior |language=en |volume=92 |issue=3 |pages=375–397 |doi=10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.02.003 |pmid=17428510 |s2cid=10730923}} which can directly affect its emotional processes.{{Cite journal |last1=Meyer |first1=Susann |last2=Puppe |first2=Birger |last3=Langbein |first3=Jan |date=November 2010 |title=[Cognitive enrichment in zoo and farm animals--implications for animal behaviour and welfare] |journal=Berliner und Munchener Tierarztliche Wochenschrift |volume=123 |issue=11–12 |pages=446–456 |issn=0005-9366 |pmid=21141273}} Cognitive enrichment should be provided in addition to a diverse environment that is already structurally and socially enriched; it goes beyond the basic needs of the animals.

= Social enrichment =

Social enrichment can either involve housing a group of conspecifics or animals of different species that would naturally encounter each other in the wild. Social animals in particular (i.e. most primates, lions, flamingos, etc.), benefit from social enrichment because it has the positive effect of creating confidence in the group.{{Cite news|url=https://www.colchester-zoo.com/explore/environmental-enrichment|title=Environmental Enrichment - Colchester Zoo|access-date=2018-03-29 }} Social enrichment can encourage social behaviors that are seen in the wild, including feeding, foraging, defense, territoriality, reproduction, and courtship.{{Cite web|url=https://littlerockzoo.com/socialenrichment|title=Social Enrichment|website=littlerockzoo.com|access-date=2018-03-29|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20180412082034/https://littlerockzoo.com/socialenrichment|archive-date=2018-04-12|url-status=dead}}

= Human-interaction enrichment =

The most common form of human-interaction enrichment is training. The human and animal interaction during training builds trust, and increases the animal's cooperation during clinical and research procedures. In addition, training sessions have been shown to benefit the welfare of both individually housed animals and communally housed animals by providing cognitive stimulation, increasing social play, decreasing inactivity, and mitigating social aggression during feeding.{{cite journal | vauthors = Baker KC | title = Benefits of Positive Human Interaction for Socially-Housed Chimpanzees | journal = Animal Welfare | volume = 13 | issue = 2 | pages = 239–245 | date = May 2004 | doi = 10.1017/S0962728600026981 | pmid = 20505791 | pmc = 2875797 }}

Assessing the success

A range of methods can be used to assess which environmental enrichment should be provided. These are based on the premises that captive animals should perform behaviors in a similar way to those in the ethogram of their ancestral species,{{cite journal | vauthors = Dawkins MS | year = 1989 | title = Time budgets in red junglefowl as a baseline for the assessment of welfare in domestic-fowl. | journal = Applied Animal Behaviour Science | volume = 24 | pages = 77–80 | doi = 10.1016/0168-1591(89)90126-3 }} animals should be allowed to perform the activities or interactions they prefer, i.e. preference test studies,{{cite journal | vauthors = Sherwin CM, Glen EF | s2cid = 53191108 | year = 2003 | title = Cage colour preferences and effects of home-cage colour on anxiety in laboratory mice. | journal = Animal Behaviour | volume = 66 | issue = 6 | pages = 1085–1092 | doi = 10.1006/anbe.2003.2286 }} and animals should be allowed to perform those activities for which they are highly motivated, i.e. motivation studies.{{cite journal | vauthors = Sherwin CM | s2cid = 53181481 | year = 2004 | title = The motivation of group-housed laboratory mice, Mus musculus, for additional space. | journal = Animal Behaviour | volume = 67 | issue = 4 | pages = 711–717 | doi = 10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.08.018 }}

Environmental enrichment is a way to ensure that an animals natural and instinctual behaviors are kept and able to be passed and taught from one generation to the next. Enrichment techniques that encourage species specific behaviors, like those that are discovered in the wild, have been studied and found to help the process of reintroduction of endangered species into their natural habitats, as well as helping to create offspring with natural traits and behaviors.{{Cite book| vauthors = Shepherdson O |date=1994-01-01|publisher=Springer Netherlands|isbn=9789401043113| veditors = Olney PJ, Mace GM, Feistner AT |pages=[https://archive.org/details/creativeconserva00magi/page/n196 167]–177|doi=10.1007/978-94-011-0721-1_8 |title = Creative Conservation| url = https://archive.org/details/creativeconserva00magi | url-access = limited |chapter = The role of environmental enrichment in the captive breeding and reintroduction of endangered species}}

The main way the success of environmental enrichment can be measured is by recognizing the behavioral changes that occur from the techniques used to shape desired behaviors of the animal compared to the behaviors of those found in the wild.{{Cite book|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=LmKCN-7kluYC&q=success+of+environmental+enrichment+in+zoos&pg=PA337|title=The Biology of Animal Stress: Basic Principles and Implications for Animal Welfare|last1=Moberg|first1=Gary P.|last2=Mench|first2=Joy A. | name-list-style = vanc |date=2000-01-01|publisher=CABI|isbn=9780851999302}} Other ways that the success of environmental enrichment can be assessed quantitatively by a range of behavioral and physiological indicators of animal welfare. In addition to those listed above, behavioral indicators include the occurrence of abnormal behaviours (e.g. stereotypies),{{cite journal | vauthors = Mason GJ | year = 1991 | title = Stereotypies - A critical review. | journal = Animal Behaviour | volume = 41 | issue = 6 | pages = 1015–1037 | doi = 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80640-2 | hdl = 10214/4622 | s2cid = 53187334 | hdl-access = free }}{{cite journal | vauthors = Claes A, Attur Shanmugam A, Jensen P | year = 2010 | title = Habituation to environmental enrichment in captive Sloth Bears—effect on stereotypies | journal = Zoo Biology | volume = 29 | issue = 6 | pages = 705–714 | doi = 10.1002/zoo.20301 | pmid = 20069629 }} cognitive bias studies,{{cite journal | vauthors = Mendl M, Burman OH, Parker RM, Paul ES | year = 2009 | title = Cognitive bias as an indicator of animal emotion and welfare: Emerging evidence and underlying mechanisms. | journal = Applied Animal Behaviour Science | volume = 118 | issue = 3–4 | pages = 161–181 | doi = 10.1016/j.applanim.2009.02.023 }} and the effects of frustration.{{cite journal | vauthors = Duncan IJ, Wood-Gush DG | year = 1971 | title = Frustration and aggression in the domestic fowl. | journal = Animal Behaviour | volume = 19 | issue = 3 | pages = 500–504 | doi = 10.1016/S0003-3472(71)80104-5 | pmid = 5167834 }}{{cite journal | vauthors = Zimmerman PH, Lundberg A, Keeling LJ, Koene P | year = 2003 | title = The effect of an audience on the gakel-call and other frustration behaviours in the laying hen (Gallus gallus domesticus) | journal = Animal Welfare | volume = 12 | issue = 3 | pages = 315–326 | doi = 10.1017/S0962728600025823 | s2cid = 145114602 }} Physiological indicators include heart rate,{{cite journal | vauthors = Kemppinen N, Hau J, Meller A, Mauranen K, Kohila T, Nevalainen T | s2cid = 17743686 | year = 2010 | title = Impact of aspen furniture and restricted feeding on activity, blood pressure, heart rate and faecal corticosterone and immunoglobulin A excretion in rats (Rattus norvegicus) housed in individually ventilated cages | journal = Laboratory Animals | volume = 44 | issue = 2 | pages = 104–112 | doi = 10.1258/la.2009.009058 | pmid = 19854757 | doi-access = free }} corticosteroids,{{cite journal | vauthors = Laws N, Ganswindt A, Heistermann M, Harris M, Harris S, Sherwin C | year = 2007 | title = A case study: fecal corticosteroid and behavior as indicators of welfare during relocation of an asian elephant. | journal = Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science | volume = 10 | issue = 4 | pages = 349–358 | doi = 10.1080/10888700701555600 | pmid = 17970634 | s2cid = 46617133 }} immune function,{{cite journal | vauthors = Martin LB, Kidd L, Liebl AL, Coon CA | title = Captivity induces hyper-inflammation in the house sparrow (Passer domesticus) | journal = The Journal of Experimental Biology | volume = 214 | issue = Pt 15 | pages = 2579–85 | date = August 2011 | pmid = 21753052 | doi = 10.1242/jeb.057216 | s2cid = 31326602 | doi-access = free }} neurobiology,{{cite book | vauthors = Lewis MH, Presti MF, Lewis JB, Turner CA | year = 2006 | chapter = The neurobiology of stereotypy I: Environmental complexity. | title = Stereotypic Animal Behaviour: Fundamentals and Applications to Welfare | veditors = Mason G, Rushen J | publisher= CABI | pages = 190–226 | doi = 10.1079/9780851990040.0190 | isbn = 9780851990040 }} eggshell quality{{cite journal | vauthors = Hughes BO, Gilbert AB, Brown MF | year = 1986 | title = Categorisation and causes of abnormal egg shells: relationship with stress. | journal = British Poultry Science | volume = 27 | issue = 2 | pages = 325–337 | doi = 10.1080/00071668608416885 | pmid = 3742268 }} and thermography.{{cite journal | vauthors = Wilcox CS, Patterson J, Cheng HW | year = 2009 | title = Use of thermography to screen for subclinical bumblefoot in poultry. | journal = Poultry Science | volume = 88 | issue = 6 | pages = 1176–1180 | doi = 10.3382/ps.2008-00446 | pmid = 19439627 | title-link = Bumblefoot (infection) | s2cid = 42216865 | doi-access = free }}

It is very difficult for zookeepers to measure the effectiveness of enrichment in terms of the stress due to the fact that animals that are found in zoos are oftentimes on display and presented with very abnormal conditions that can cause uneasiness and stress. Measuring enrichment in terms of reproduction is easier because of our ability to record offspring numbers and fertility. By making necessary environment changes and providing mental stimulation, animals in captivity have been seen to reproduce at a more similar rate to their wild ancestors in comparison to those provided with less behavioral and environmental enrichment.

Issues and concerns

= Habituation =

Although environmental enrichment can provide sensory and social stimulations, it can also have limited efficacy if not changed frequently. Animals can become habituated to environmental enrichments, showing positive behaviors at onset of exposure and progressively declining with time. Environmental enrichments are effective primarily because it offers novelty stimuli, making the animal's daily routines less predictable, as would be in the wild. Therefore, maintaining novelty is important for the efficacy of the enrichment. Frequently changing the type of environmental enrichment will help prevent habituation.

= Training =

Usage of more highly advanced enrichment devices, such as computerized devices, requires training. This can lead to issues as training often consists of food as a reward. While food encourages the animal to participate with the device, the animal could associate the device with food. As a result, the interaction with the enrichment would bring about behaviors that are associated with training instead of the desired playful and voluntary behaviors.{{cite journal | vauthors = French F, Mancini C, Sharp H | title = High tech cognitive and acoustic enrichment for captive elephants | journal = Journal of Neuroscience Methods | volume = 300 | pages = 173–183 | date = April 2018 | pmid = 28951175 | doi = 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2017.09.009 | s2cid = 4548453 | url = http://repository.londonmet.ac.uk/3437/7/high_tech_elephant_Dec_2017.pdf }}

= Time and resources =

The process of producing and providing environmental enrichment usually require a large allocation of time and resources. In a survey,{{cite journal |vauthors=Hoy JM, Murray PJ, Tribe A |date=2010 |title=Thirty years later: enrichment practices for captive mammals |url=https://www.researchgate.net/publication/24420182 |journal=Zoo Biology |volume=29 |issue=3 |pages=303–16 |doi=10.1002/zoo.20254 |pmid=19434736}} "time taken by animal care staff to complete other tasks" was the most significant factor influencing environmental enrichment provisions and scheduling. Therefore, it is important to develop appropriate environmental enrichment programs that can be effectively carried out with the size of staff and time available.

See also

References

{{Reflist|32em}}