Jean Decety
{{Short description|American neuroscientist}}
{{Use mdy dates|date=March 2025}}
{{Infobox scientist
| image = Jean Decety University of Chicago September 2015 (cropped).jpg
| image_size =
| name = Jean Decety
| birth_date = {{bya|1960}}
| birth_place = France
| death_date =
| death_place =
| nationality = French and American
| field = Cognitive neuroscience, Developmental neuroscience, Social neuroscience
| work_institution = University of Chicago (Professor)
| alma_mater = Université Claude Bernard, Lyon; France
| notable_students = Sarah-Jayne Blakemore{{Cite journal
| doi = 10.1038/35086023
| title = REVIEW: From the perception of action to the understanding of intention
| journal = Nature Reviews Neuroscience
| volume = 2
| issue = 8
| pages = 561–567
| year = 2001
| last1 = Blakemore | first1 = S.
| authorlink1 = Sarah-Jayne Blakemore
| last2 = Decety | first2 = J.
| authorlink2 = Jean Decety
| pmid=11483999
| s2cid = 205019257
}}
| known_for = {{Plainlist|
| prizes =
| footnotes =
}}
Jean Decety is an American–French neuroscientist specializing in developmental neuroscience, affective neuroscience, and social neuroscience. His research focuses on the psychological and neurobiological mechanisms underpinning social cognition, particularly social decision-making, empathy, moral reasoning, altruism, pro-social behavior, and more generally interpersonal relationships. He is Irving B. Harris Distinguished Service Professor at the University of Chicago.
Background
Jean Decety obtained three advanced master's degrees in 1985 (neuroscience), in 1986 (cognitive psychology), and in 1987 (biomedical engineering science) and was awarded a Ph.D. in 1989 (neurobiology - medicine) from the Université Claude Bernard. After receiving his doctorate, he worked as a post-doctoral fellow at the hospital in Lund (Sweden) in the Department of Neurophysiology, then in the Karolinska Hospital, Stockholm (Sweden) in the Departments of Neurophysiology and Neuroradiology. He then joined the National Institute for Medical Research (INSERM) in Lyon (France) until 2001.
Decety is currently professor at the University of Chicago and the College, with appointments in the Department of Psychology, and in the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neuroscience. He is the Director of the Social Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory, and the Child NeuroSuite. Decety is a member of the Committee on Computational Neuroscience and the Center for Integrative Neuroscience and Neuroengineering. In 2022, Decety was elected as a member of the Academia Europaea, a pan-European Academy of Humanities, Letters, Law, and Sciences, in the Physiology and Neuroscience section.{{cite web | url=https://www.ae-info.org/ae/Member/Decety_Jean | title=Academy of Europe: Decety Jean }}
Editorial activities
Decety served as the founder and editor-in-chief of the journal Social Neuroscience between 2006 and 2012, and he is on the editorial boards of Development and Psychopathology, The European Journal of Neuroscience, The Scientific World Journal, Frontiers in Emotion Science, and Neuropsychologia. With his colleague John Cacioppo, Decety played an instrumental role in the creation of the Society for Social Neuroscience in 2010.
Mental simulation of actions
Mental simulation, also known as motor imagery, mental practice, or mental rehearsal, refers to the human cognitive ability to imagine doing a specific action or behavior and simulating the probable outcome before acting. It has been part of elite sports training for a long time. [https://www.nytimes.com/2014/02/23/sports/olympics/olympians-use-imagery-as-mental-training.html Olympians use imagery as mental training] Research pioneered by Decety using psychophysics, functional neuroimaging, H-reflex excitability, as well as measures of the autonomic nervous system, demonstrated that imagining an action activates similar neural representations that would be engaged by carrying out the same action.Decety, J. (1996). The neurophysiological basis of motor imagery. Behavioural Brain Research, 77(1-2), 45-52. For instance, an increase in heart rate and respiratory rate is proportional to the level of mental effort in athletes who imagine running on a treadmill at different speeds.Decety, J., Jeannerod, M., Germain, M., & Pastene, J. (1991). Vegetative response during imagined movement is proportional to mental effort. Behavioural Brain Research, 42(1), 1-5. Imagining doing an action is associated with activation of the supplementary motor area, parietal cortex, somatosensory cortex, and cerebellum, brain regions involved in motor control.Decety, J. (1996). The neurophysiological basis of motor imagery. Behavioural Brain Research, 77(1-2), 45-52. Together, these findings have been interpreted as a demonstration of functional equivalence between the imagination and the production of action, to the extent that they share the same motor representations underpinned by the same neurophysiological substrate.Decety, J., & Grèzes, J. (2006). The power of simulation: imagining one's own and other's behavior. Brain Research, 1079(1), 4-14. This theoretical framework was then extended to empathy and some aspects of social cognition.Jackson, P. L., & Decety, J. (2004). Motor cognition: A new paradigm to study self–other interactions. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 14(2), 259-263.Decety, J., & Jackson, P. L. (2004). The functional architecture of human empathy. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 3(2), 71-100.
Empathy, moral reasoning and prosocial behavior
Decety studies the neurobiological and psychological mechanisms that guide social decision-making, moral reasoning, empathy and sensitivity for justice, as well as how these abilities develop in children, and are shaped by life experiences and group dynamics. Decety conducts research on various aspects of empathy, including its evolutionary origins,Decety, J. (2011). The neuroevolution of empathy. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1231, 35-45.Decety, J., Norman, G. J., Berntson, G. G., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2012). A neurobehavioral evolutionary perspective on the mechanisms underlying empathy. Progress in Neurobiology, 98, 38-48. its development in young children,Decety, J. (2010). The neurodevelopment of empathy in humans. Developmental Neuroscience, 32, 257-267. as well as how empathy is modulated by the social environment and interpersonal relationships.Decety, J., Echols, S. C., & Correll, J. (2010). The blame game: the effect of responsibility and social stigma on empathy for pain. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 985-997.Cheng, Y., Chen, C. Y., Lin, C. P., Chou, K. H., & Decety, J. (2010). Love hurts: an fMRI study. NeuroImage, 51, 923-929.
Decety investigates the development of moral behavior, generosity and distributive justice in children across South East Asia, Europe, the Middle East, North and South America, and South Africa.Cowell, J. M., Lee, L., Malcolm-Smith, S., Selcuk, B., Zhou, X., & Decety, J. (2016). The development of generosity and moral cognition across five cultures. Developmental Science, epub ahead of print.Cowell, J. M., & Decety, J. (2015). The neuroscience of implicit moral evaluation and its relation to generosity in early childhood. Current Biology, 25, 1-5.Li, Y., Li, H., Decety, J., & Lee, K. (2013). Experiencing a natural disaster alters children's altruistic giving. Psychological Science, 24(9), 1686-1695.Yoder, K. J., & Decety, J. (2014). The good, the bad, and the just: Justice sensitivity predicts neural response during moral evaluation of actions performed by others. The Journal of Neuroscience, 34(12), 4161-4166. He argues that empathy is not necessarily a direct avenue to moral behavior, and that it can lead to immoral behavior.Decety, J., & Cowell, J. M. (2014). Friends or foes: Is empathy necessary for moral behavior? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9(5), 525-537. The influence that empathy and justice exert on one another is complex, and empathy can induce partiality and threaten justice principles.Decety, J., & Cowell, J. M. (2014). The complex relation between morality and empathy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(7), 337-339.
Based on empirical research combining functional neuroimaging (fMRI and EEG), developmental psychology, and individual differences in personality traits, Decety argues that in order to promote justice, it may be more effective to encourage perspective taking and reasoning than emphasizing emotional sharing with the misfortune of others.Decety, J., & Yoder, K. J. (2015). Empathy and motivation for justice: Cognitive empathy and concern, but not emotional empathy, predicts sensitivity to injustice for others. Social Neuroscience, epub ahead of print.Yoder, K., J., & Decety, J. (2014). Spatiotemporal neural dynamics of moral judgments: A high-density EEG/ERP study. Neuropsychologia, 60, 39-45.
Decety thinks that the ability to recognize and vicariously experience what another individual is undergoing was a key step forward in the evolution of social behavior, and ultimately, morality.{{Cite news| url=https://www.washingtonpost.com/wp-dyn/content/article/2007/05/27/AR2007052701056_pf.html | newspaper=The Washington Post | title=If It Feels Good to Be Good, It Might Be Only Natural | first=Shankar | last=Vedantam | access-date=May 13, 2010}} The inability to feel empathy is one of the defining characteristics of psychopathy, and this would appear to lend support to Decety's view.{{Cite journal|vauthors=de Wied M, Goudena PP, Matthys W |title=Empathy in boys with disruptive behavior disorders |journal=Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines |volume=46 |issue=8 |pages=867–80 |year=2005 |pmid=16033635 |doi=10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00389.x|hdl=1874/11212 |s2cid=45683502 |hdl-access=free }}{{Cite journal|vauthors=Fernandez YM, Marshall WL |title=Victim empathy, social self-esteem, and psychopathy in rapists |journal=Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment |volume=15 |issue=1 |pages=11–26 |year=2003 |pmid=12616926 |doi=10.1023/A:1020611606754|s2cid=195293070 |doi-access=free }}
While empathy plays an important role in motivating caring for others and in guiding moral behavior, Decety's research demonstrates that this is far from being systematic or irrespective to the social identity of the targets, interpersonal relationships, and social context. He proposes that empathic concern (compassion) has evolved to favor kin and members of one own social group, can bias social decision-making by valuing one single individual over a group of others, and this can frontally conflict with principles of fairness and justice.Decety, J., & Cowell, J. M. (2015). Empathy, justice and moral behavior. American Journal of Bioethics – Neuroscience, 6(3), 3-14.
Recently, drawing on empirical research in evolutionary theory, developmental psychology, social neuroscience, and psychopathy, Jean Decety argued that empathy and morality are neither systematically opposed to one another, nor inevitably complementary.{{cite book|last=Decety|first=Jean|series=Research and Perspectives in Neurosciences |title=New Frontiers in Social Neuroscience |date=November 1, 2014|chapter=The Neuroevolution of Empathy and Caring for Others: Why It Matters for Morality|chapter-url=https://www.researchgate.net/publication/287290900|volume=21|pages=127–151|doi=10.1007/978-3-319-02904-7_8|isbn=978-3-319-02903-0}}{{cite journal|last1=Decety|first1=J.|last2=Cowell|first2=J. M.|year=2014|title=The complex relation between morality and empathy|url=http://spihub.org/site/resource_files/publications/spi_wp_135_decety.pdf|journal=Trends in Cognitive Sciences|volume=18|issue=7|pages=337–339|doi=10.1016/j.tics.2014.04.008|pmid=24972506|s2cid=355141|access-date=February 25, 2021|archive-date=October 23, 2022|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20221023104046/https://spihub.org/site/resource_files/publications/spi_wp_135_decety.pdf|url-status=dead}}
Empathy and psychopathy
A lack of empathy is a hallmark characteristic of psychopathy. As a consequence, Decety investigates atypical socioemotional processing and moral judgment in forensic psychopaths with a mobile MRI scanner, because they provide a natural model in which emotional and attentional processes are altered, enabling identification of downstream effects, including the extent to which empathy is a critical input for caring.Decety, J., Chen, C., Harenski, C. L., & Kiehl, K. A. (2013). An fMRI study of affective perspective taking in individuals with psychopathy: imagining another in pain does not evoke empathy. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 489. His work shows that the higher the level of psychopathy, the less neural activity in ventromedial prefrontal cortex in response to perceiving interpersonal harm as well as expressions of physical and emotional pain.Decety, J., Skelly, L. R., & Kiehl, K. A. (2013). Brain response to empathy-eliciting scenarios in incarcerated individuals with psychopathy. JAMA Psychiatry, 70(6), 638-645.Decety, L., Skelly, L. R., Yoder, K. J., & Kiehl, K. (2014). Neural processing of dynamic facial expressions in psychopaths. Social Neuroscience, 9, 36-49. This region is attributed with various functions related to valuation, affect regulation and social cognition.Delgado, M. R., Beer, J. S., Fellows, L. K., Huettel, S. A., Platt, M. L., Quirk, G. J., & Schiller, D. (2016). Viewpoints: dialogues on the functional role of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 19(12), 1545-1552.
Moral conviction
Decety started a new line of inquiry into characterizing the neural mechanisms of what he calls "the dark side of morality," in particular, the role of moral conviction in justifying violence.Workman, C. I., Yoder, K. J., & Decety, J. (2020). The dark side of morality – Neural mechanisms underpinning moral convictions and support for violence. American Journal of Bioethics Neuroscience, 11(4), 269-284. While violence is often described as antithetical to sociality, it can be motivated by moral values with the ultimate goal of regulating social relationships, as shown by the work of Alan Fiske.Fiske, A. P., & Rai, T. S. (2014). Virtuous violence: Hurting and killing to create, sustain, end, and honor social relationships. Cambridge University Press. In fact, most violence in the world appears to be rooted in conflict between moral values.Rai, T. S., & Fiske, A. P. (2011). Moral psychology is relationship regulation: moral motives for unity, hierarchy, equality, and proportionality. Psychological review, 118(1), 57.
Moral development across cultures
To understand how morality emerges from the interaction between innate predispositions, shaped by evolution and input from local cultural environments, Decety conducts empirical research on the development of moral cognition and its relation to prosociality across different countries using behavioral economics games.
A first study combined measures of socioeconomic status (SES), executive functions, affective sharing, empathy, theory of mind, and moral judgment in predicting altruism in children from the age of 5 to 12 in five large‐scale societies: Canada, China, Turkey, South Africa, and the US.Cowell, J. M., Lee, L., Malcolm-Smith, S., Selcuk, B., Zhou, X., & Decety, J. (2017). The development of generosity and moral cognition across five cultures. Developmental Science, doi: 10.1111/desc.12403. Results demonstrate that age, gender, SES, and cognitive processes (executive function and theory of mind), but not empathy, were the best predictor of children's generosity in a costly resource allocation game. Such results fit well with a growing literature suggesting that advanced theory of mind and executive functioning promote moral behavior.Imuta, K., Henry, J. D., Slaughter, V., Selcuk, B., & Ruffman, T. (2016). Theory of mind and prosocial behavior in childhood: A meta-analytic review. Developmental Psychology, 52(8), 1192-1205.
A second study examined the extent to which social norms are integrated into fairness considerations and how they influence social preferences regarding equality and equity in a large sample of children in Argentina, Canada, Chile, China, Colombia, Cuba, Jordan, Mexico, Norway, South Africa, Taiwan, Turkey, and the US. Social decision-making in distributive justice games revealed universal developmental shifts from equality-based to equity-based distribution decisions across cultures.Huppert, E., Cowell, J. M., Cheng, Y., Contreras, C., Gomez-Sicard, N., Gonzalez-Gaeda, L. M., Huepe, D., Ibanez, A., Lee, K., Mahasneh, R., Malcolm-Smith, S., Salas, N., Selcuk, B., Tungodden, B., Wong, A., Zhou, X., & Decety, J. (2018). The development of children's preferences for equality and equity across 13 individualistic and collectivist cultures. Developmental Science, epub ahead of print. However, differences in levels of individualism and collectivism between the countries, classified by the Hofstede scale, predicted the age and extent to which children favor equity for recipients differing in terms of wealth, merit, and physical suffering to elicit empathy. Children from the most individualistic cultures endorsed equitable distributions to a greater degree than children from more collectivist cultures when recipients differed in regards to wealth and merit. Children from the more individualistic cultures also favored equitable distribution at an earlier age than children from more collectivist cultures overall. These results provide insights into theories positing that fairness is a universal moral concern, and that humans naturally favor fair distributions, not equal ones.Starmans, C., Sheskin, M., Bloom, P., Christakis, N. A., & Brown, G. D. (2017). Why people prefer unequal societies. Nature Human Behaviour, 1(4), 82.Baumard, N., André, J. B., & Sperber, D. (2013). A mutualistic approach to morality: The evolution of fairness by partner choice. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(1), 59-78. They suggest commonalities in children's development of fairness conceptualizations and preferences across diverse cultures. However, social learning within a culture does appear to affect some aspects of prosociality and concern for equity.
Controversies
In 2015 Decety published a study examining religion and morality in children that concluded "that children from households identifying as either of the two major world religions (Christianity and Islam) were less altruistic than children from non-religious households." The study utilized behavioral measures of punitive tendencies when evaluating interpersonal harm, moral judgment, empathy, and generosity (Dictator Game) in 1,151 children aged 5–12 years sampled from six countries (Canada, China, Jordan, South Africa, Turkey and USA). The authors found that children from religious households believe that interpersonal harm is more "mean" and deserving of harsher punishment than non-religious children.{{Cite journal|last1=Decety|first1=Jean|last2=Cowell|first2=Jason M.|last3=Lee|first3=Kang|last4=Mahasneh|first4=Randa|last5=Malcolm-Smith|first5=Susan|last6=Selcuk|first6=Bilge|last7=Zhou|first7=Xinyue|date=November 16, 2015|title=The Negative Association between Religiousness and Children's Altruism across the World|journal=Current Biology|volume=25|issue=22|pages=2951–2955|doi=10.1016/j.cub.2015.09.056|pmid=26549259|doi-access=free|bibcode=2015CBio...25.2951D }}{{Retracted|doi=10.1016/j.cub.2019.07.030|pmid=31386841|intentional=yes}} They also reported that religiousness was inversely predictive of children's altruism at least when generosity is spontaneously directed to an anonymous beneficiary. The study received widespread attention from news outlets and social media, with news outlets citing it as evidence that religious children are more selfish than their secular counterparts.{{cite web|url=https://www.altmetric.com/details/4728795|title=Altmetric – The Negative Association between Religiousness and Children's Altruism across the World|website=www.altmetric.com|access-date=October 23, 2017}}{{Cite news|url=https://www.theguardian.com/world/2015/nov/06/religious-children-less-altruistic-secular-kids-study|title=Religious children are meaner than their secular counterparts, study finds|last=correspondent|first=Harriet Sherwood Religion|date=November 6, 2015|work=The Guardian|access-date=October 23, 2017|language=en-GB|issn=0261-3077}}{{Cite news|url=http://www.slate.com/articles/health_and_science/science/2015/11/religious_children_are_more_selfish_in_a_sticker_study.html|title=Are Religious Children More Selfish?|last=Gross|first=Rachel E.|date=November 6, 2015|work=Slate|access-date=October 23, 2017|language=en-US|issn=1091-2339}}
However, Decety has retracted the study, citing an analysis error which nullified the studies conclusion on religiousness and altruism.{{cite journal |last1=Decety |first1=J |last2=Cowell |first2=JM |last3=Lee |first3=K |last4=Mahasneh |first4=R |last5=Malcolm-Smith |first5=S |last6=Selcuk |first6=B |last7=Zhou |first7=X |title=Retraction Notice to: The Negative Association between Religiousness and Children's Altruism across the World. |journal=Current Biology |date=August 5, 2019 |volume=29 |issue=15 |pages=2595 |doi=10.1016/j.cub.2019.07.030 |pmid=31386841|doi-access=free |bibcode=2019CBio...29.2595D }} This came about after Azim F. Shariff reanalyzed the Decety's original data and observed that there was a mistake in the original study, coding country as a continuous rather than categorical variable. Once this error was corrected, as Shariff wrote, "most of the associations they observed with religious affiliation appear to be artifacts of between-country differences, driven primarily by low levels of generosity in Turkey and South Africa. However, children from highly religious households do appear slightly less generous than those from moderately religious ones."{{Cite journal|last1=Shariff|first1=Azim F.|last2=Willard|first2=Aiyana K.|last3=Muthukrishna|first3=Michael|last4=Kramer|first4=Stephanie R.|last5=Henrich|first5=Joseph|date=August 8, 2016|title=What is the association between religious affiliation and children's altruism?|journal=Current Biology|volume=26|issue=15|pages=R699–R700|doi=10.1016/j.cub.2016.06.031|pmid=27505237|doi-access=free|bibcode=2016CBio...26.R699S }} Decety has stated "When we reanalyzed these data to correct this error, we found that country of origin, rather than religious affiliation, is the primary predictor of several of the outcomes."{{cite journal |last1=Decety |first1=J |last2=Cowell |first2=JM |last3=Lee |first3=K |last4=Mahasneh |first4=R |last5=Malcolm-Smith |first5=S |last6=Selcuk |first6=B |last7=Zhou |first7=X |title=Retraction Notice to: The Negative Association between Religiousness and Children's Altruism across the World. |journal=Current Biology |date=August 5, 2019 |volume=29 |issue=15 |pages=2595 |doi=10.1016/j.cub.2019.07.030 |pmid=31386841|doi-access=free |bibcode=2019CBio...29.2595D }}
Edited books
- Social Cognition: Development Across the Life Span (2017). Jessica A. Sommerville and Jean Decety (Eds). New York: Routledge.
- The Moral Brain: A Multidisciplinary Perspective (2015). Jean Decety and Thalia Wheatley (Eds). Cambridge: MIT Press.
- New Frontiers in Social Neuroscience (2014). Jean Decety and Yves Christen (Eds). Berlin: Springer.
- Empathy - from Bench to Bedside (2012). Jean Decety (Ed). Cambridge: MIT Press, Cambridge.
- The Oxford Handbook of Social Neuroscience (2011). Jean Decety and John T. Cacioppo (Eds). New York: Oxford University Press.
- The Social Neuroscience of Empathy (2009). Jean Decety and William Ickes (Eds). Cambridge: MIT Press, Cambridge.
- Interpersonal Sensitivity: Entering Others' Worlds (2007). Jean Decety and C. Daniel Batson (Eds). Hove: Psychology Press.
References
{{reflist}}
External links
{{Commons category}}
- [http://S4SN.org/ Society for Social Neuroscience home page]
- [https://news.uchicago.edu/profile/jean-decety/ University of Chicago News]
- [http://childneurosuite.org/ University of Chicago Child Neurosuite]
- [https://news.uchicago.edu/article/2010/12/13/new-society-social-neuroscience-help-guide-emerging-field/ Establishing a Society for Social Neuroscience]
- [https://mag.uchicago.edu/law-policy-society/society-social-neuroscience/ A New Society for Social Neuroscience]
{{Authority control}}
{{DEFAULTSORT:Decety, Jean}}
Category:University of Chicago faculty
Category:American neuroscientists
Category:Fellows of the American Association for the Advancement of Science
Category:French people of Jewish descent
Category:French neuroscientists
Category:21st-century French psychologists
Category:Cognitive neuroscientists
Category:American moral psychologists