Lizard communication

{{Short description|Sending or receiving of information as conducted by lizards}}

Lizards are among the most diverse groups of reptiles, with more than 5,600 species. With such diversity in physical and behavioral traits, lizards have evolved many ways to communicate. Communication may be physical, chemical, tactile, or vocal, and varies according to habitat, sexual selection practices, and predator avoidance methods. Each type of communication uses different sensory systems, including visual, olfactory, and auditory.Fleishman, L J, M Bowman, D Saunders, WE Miller, MJ Rury, and ER Loew (1997). The visual ecology of Puerto Rican anoline lizards: habitat light and spectral.Halpern, M "Nasal Chemical Senses in Reptiles: Structure and Function." Biology of the Reptilia: Hormones, Brain, and Behavior. Ed. D Crews. Vol. 18. Chicago and London: U of Chicago, 1992. 423–525. Print. Physiology E.Manley, GA (2011). Lizard auditory papillae: An evolutionary kaleidoscope. Hearing research, 273(1), 59–64.

Scenarios for lizard communication

Lizards communicate with members of their own species{{Cite journal |last1=Campos |first1=Stephanie M |last2=Belkasim |first2=Selma S |date=2021 |title=Chemical Communication in Lizards and a Potential Role for Vasotocin in Modulating Social Interactions |journal=Integrative & Comparative Biology |volume=61 |issue=1 |pages=205–220 |doi=10.1093/icb/icab044|pmid=33940600 }} to find appropriate mates{{Cite web |title=Reptile - Courtship, Fertilization, Reproduction {{!}} Britannica |url=https://www.britannica.com/animal/reptile/Courtship-and-fertilization |access-date=2023-11-05 |website=www.britannica.com |language=en}} and when competing for resources such as food or habitat. Intraspecific communication is instrumental in sexual selection, as phenotypic variation within a population can result in some individuals mating more than others.

Lizard species use interspecific communication in competition for resources and during predator-prey interactions. Anolis cristatellus, for example, uses "pushup displays" as a pursuit-deterrent signal to predators, communicating that the anole is healthy and would likely be able to escape if pursued. This encourages the predator to go after other prey (potentially anoles of the same species in worse condition). Lizards in better condition perform this behavior more frequently, making it a form of honest signaling.Leal, M. (1999). Honest signaling during prey–predator interactions in the lizard Anolis cristatelwlus. Animal Behaviour, 58(3), 521–526.

Methods of communication

Lizards have evolved several modes of communication, including visual, chemical, tactile, and vocal. Chemical and visual communication are widespread, with visual communication being the most well-studied, while tactile and vocal communication have traditionally been thought to occur in just a handful of lizard species; however, modern scientific techniques have allowed for greater study of non-visual modes of communication, and they may be more widespread than previously thought.{{citation needed|date=July 2024}}

=Visual communication=

File:Blue-Eyed Anole, Corcovado Natl Park, Costa Rica.ogv lizard in Costa Rica repeatedly protracting and retracting its dewlap.]]

Lizards that use visual communication gather information by observing other lizards' physical and behavioral characteristics, somewhat like humans communicating using body language. Lizards that use visual communication often have highly developed visual systems—most can see colors, and some can see UV light.Fleishman, LJ, ER Loew, and M Leal. "Ultraviolet vision in lizards." (1993): 397-397.Whiting, MJ, DM Stuart-Fox, D O'Connor, D Firth, NC Bennett, and SP Blomberg (2006). Ultraviolet signals ultra-aggression in a lizard. Animal Behaviour, 72(2), 353–363.

Lizards can have vibrant colors and patterns and flashy behaviors intended to communicate both inter- and intraspecifically. Vibrant colors and patterns can draw attention from predators or competition, so they may be located on a dewlap or an area of the body that is usually hidden, like the belly. Many Anolis species have dewlapsLosos, JB (2009). Lizards in an evolutionary tree: ecology and adaptive radiation of anoles. University of California Press. that they can extend and retract at will, such as when attracting mates or dueling with competitors.Nicholson, KE, LJ Harmon, and JB Losos. "Evolution of Anolis lizard dewlap diversity." PLoS One 2.3 (2007): e274. The dewlaps are often colorful but are well hidden when retracted. Some Sceloporus lizards develop vibrant blue and black coloration on their bellies during breeding season.Cooper Jr, WE and N Burns (1987). Social significance of ventrolateral coloration in the fence lizard, Sceloporus undulatus. Animal Behaviour,35(2), 526–532. This color is not visible to other lizards unless the lizard flattens its body to expose the colorful parts of its belly, a behavior called dorsal ventral flattening.Martins, EP (1994). Structural complexity in a lizard communication system: the Sceloporus graciosus" push-up" display. Copeia, 944–955.

Visual communication is common among species that live at relatively high population densities and often come into close contact. Visual communication is well suited for use in many different habitats and can be modified by lizards to accommodate changing habitat conditions,Ord, TJ, RA Peters, B Clucas, and JA Stamps (2007). Lizards speed up visual displays in noisy motion habitats. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 274(1613), 1057–1062. so long as individuals come into contact frequently enough.{{clarification needed|date=July 2024}}

=Chemical communication=

Some lizards deposit chemicals such as pheromones into the environment, where the chemicals can elicit changes in the behavior and sometimes physiology of other individuals.Mason, RT "Reptilian Pheromones." Biology of the Reptilia: Hormones, Brain, and Behavior. Ed. C Gans and D Crews. Vol. 18. Chicago and London: U of Chicago, 1992. 114–228. Print. Physiology E. The composition of these chemicals often differs considerably between species, allowing lizards to tell whether the lizard that deposited the chemical was a member of the same or a different species. Chemical secretions in some species (such as Iberian rock-lizards, Lacerta monticola) also vary by individual, so individuals can tell whether the lizard that produced a chemical is familiar or not.Johansson, BG, and TM Jones (2007). The role of chemical communication in mate choice. Biological Reviews, 82(2), 265–289.

Lizards that use chemical communication have highly developed olfactory systems to detect chemicals in the environment,Halpern, M "Nasal Chemical Senses in Reptiles: Structure and Function." Biology of the Reptilia: Hormones, Brain, and Behavior. Ed. D Crews. Vol. 18. Chicago and London: U of Chicago, 1992. 423–525. Print. Physiology E. and often have femoral glands or pores on their back legsCole, CJ (1966). Femoral glands in lizards: a review. Herpetologica, 199–206. to release chemicals. Such lizards sometimes drag their back legs or the lower half of their body against a surface to spread their chemical secretions. Tongue flicking is used to "taste" chemicals in the air and on various surfaces, such as rocks or logs that another lizard might have been sitting on. Chemicals can also be deposited through feces—lizards have been observed defecating systematically throughout the area they live in, possibly chemically marking the boundaries of their territory.

Lizards do not need to come into direct contact to communicate chemically.Mason, RT, and MR Parker (2010). Social behavior and pheromonal communication in reptiles. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 196(10), 729–749. Once a chemical has been released onto a surface, it stays until washed away or otherwise removed, and lizards may come into contact with it hours or days later. Thus, chemical communication is useful among lizards that live at relatively low population densities or do not come into close contact, and it is usually better suited to dry environments, as water can wash chemicals away.

Recent advances have dramatically increased our understanding of lizard chemical communication.{{Cite journal|last1=Baeckens|first1=Simon|last2=García-Roa|first2=Roberto|last3=Martín|first3=José|last4=Van Damme|first4=Raoul|date=2017-09-01|title=The Role of Diet in Shaping the Chemical Signal Design of Lacertid Lizards|journal=Journal of Chemical Ecology|language=en|volume=43|issue=9|pages=902–910|doi=10.1007/s10886-017-0884-2|pmid=28918590 |bibcode=2017JCEco..43..902B |hdl=10067/1460940151162165141|s2cid=19088192|hdl-access=free}}{{Cite journal|last1=Baeckens|first1=Simon|last2=Martín|first2=José|last3=García-Roa|first3=Roberto|last4=van Damme|first4=Raoul|date=2018-06-14|title=Sexual selection and the chemical signal design of lacertid lizards|journal=Zoological Journal of the Linnean Society|language=en|volume=183|issue=2|pages=445–457|doi=10.1093/zoolinnean/zlx075 |hdl=10067/1475550151162165141|hdl-access=free}}{{Cite journal|last1=Baeckens|first1=Simon|last2=Martín|first2=José|last3=García-Roa|first3=Roberto|last4=Pafilis|first4=Panayiotis|last5=Huyghe|first5=Katleen|last6=Damme|first6=Raoul Van|date=2018|title=Environmental conditions shape the chemical signal design of lizards|journal=Functional Ecology|language=en|volume=32|issue=2|pages=566–580|doi=10.1111/1365-2435.12984 |doi-access=free|bibcode=2018FuEco..32..566B |hdl=10067/1475540151162165141|hdl-access=free}} Further analysis of the composition of these chemicals will yield more information about the individuals that produce them and how they communicate in different contexts.Carazo, P, E Font, and E Desfilis (2007). Chemosensory assessment of rival competitive ability and scent-mark function in a lizard, Podarcis hispanica. Animal Behaviour, 74(4), 895–902.Labra, A. (2006). Chemoreception and the assessment of fighting abilities in the lizard Liolaemus monticola. Ethology, 112(10), 993–999.Martín, J, and P López. (2007). Scent may signal fighting ability in male Iberian rock lizards. Biology Letters, 3(2), 125–127.

== In ''Podarcis hispanicus'' ==

The chemicals secreted by Podarcis hispanicus are generally more volatile and chemically stable than those of similar species. Chemosensory recognition is greater in males, as it allows them to identify specific females during breeding season and to recognize known neighbors and thus not perform anti-predator behaviors. It is also used in intra-sexual aggression. Male lizards release chemical stimuli from their femoral glands.{{Cite journal|last1=Martín|first1=José|last2=López|first2=Pilar|date=2006-03-01|title=Interpopulational differences in chemical composition and chemosensory recognition of femoral gland secretions of male lizards Podarcis hispanica: implications for sexual isolation in a species complex|url=https://doi.org/10.1007/s00049-005-0326-4|journal=Chemoecology|language=en|volume=16|issue=1|pages=31–38|doi=10.1007/s00049-005-0326-4|bibcode=2006Checo..16...31M |s2cid=32881016|issn=1423-0445}}

=Tactile communication=

Some lizards use direct or indirect touch as a form of communication. Direct contact can occur in courtship, such as through nudging or licking, and in aggressive behavior—for many lizards, a fight may escalate to biting or bumping into one another. Species that engage in physical conflict often only resort to direct contact when other methods of deterring potential rivals have failed. In the case of courtship, some lizards, such as the male Komodo dragon (Varanus komodoensis), lick females to determine whether they are sexually receptive.Ciofi, C (1999). The Komodo dragon. Scientific American, 280(3), 84–91. While this is direct touch, the male is also detecting chemicals in the female's body.

Some forms of tactile communication, such as vibrational, do not involve direct touch.Markl, H (1983). Vibrational communication. In Neuroethology and behavioral physiology (pp. 332–353). Springer Berlin Heidelberg. Some chameleon species communicate with one another by vibrating the substrate they are standing on, such as a tree branch or leaf.Barnett, KE , RB Cocroft, and LJ Fleishman. (1999). Possible communication by substrate vibration in a chameleon. Copeia, 225–228. Unique adaptations in ear and jaw morphology enable such lizards to detect these vibrations.Hill, PS (2001). Vibration and animal communication: a review. American Zoologist, 41(5), 1135–1142. Lizards that live on easily moved substrates such as thin tree branches or leaves are more likely to use vibrational communication than lizards that live on substrates that do not transmit vibrations as easily, such as the ground or thick tree trunks.

=Vocal communication=

File:Tokay gecko @Vnm.jpg is known for its vocalizations]]

This mode of communication is primarily limited to nocturnal geckos, many of which produce vocalizations during behavioral interactions such as male competition{{cite journal |last1=Frankenberg |first1=Eliezer |title=Vocal behavior of the Mediterranean house gecko, Hemidactylus turcicus |journal=Copeia |date=1982 |volume=1982 |issue=4 |pages=770–775 |doi=10.2307/1444085 |jstor=1444085 }}{{cite journal |last1=Hibbitts |first1=Toby J. |last2=Whiting |first2=Martin J. |last3=Stuart-Fox |first3=Devi M. |title=Shouting the odds: vocalization signals status in a lizard |journal=Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology |date=2007 |volume=61 |issue=8 |pages=1169–1176 |doi=10.1007/s00265-006-0330-x|bibcode=2007BEcoS..61.1169H |s2cid=12740306 }} or predator avoidance.{{Cite journal|last1=Baeckens|first1=Simon|last2=Llusia|first2=Diego|last3=García-Roa|first3=Roberto|last4=Martín|first4=José|date=2019-05-29|title=Lizard calls convey honest information on body size and bite performance: a role in predator deterrence?|journal=Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology|language=en|volume=73|issue=6|pages=87|doi=10.1007/s00265-019-2695-7 |bibcode=2019BEcoS..73...87B |hdl=10067/1598890151162165141|s2cid=169033841|hdl-access=free}} Another lizard, Liolaemus chiliensis, emits distress calls. Other lizards can produce vocalizations—for instance, when handled—but most have not been observed to do so in the wild.

Lizards that use vocal communication need to produce vocalizations and need an appropriate auditory system to process the sounds.{{explain|date=July 2024}} Vocal communication is well suited for nocturnal lizards or those that live in low-visibility habitats because it does not require coming into contact with or seeing other lizards.

{{-}}

References

{{Reflist|refs=

{{cite journal |last1=Aragón |first1=Pedro |last2=López |first2=Pilar |last3=Martín |first3=José |title=Chemosensory discrimination of familiar and unfamiliar conspecifics by lizards: implications of field spatial relationships between males |journal=Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology |date=2001 |volume=50 |issue=2 |pages=128–133 |doi=10.1007/s002650100344 |jstor=4601945 |bibcode=2001BEcoS..50..128A |s2cid=35772031 }}

{{cite journal |last1=Ferguson |first1=Gary W. |title=Display and communications in reptiles: an historical perspective |journal=American Zoologist |date=1977 |volume=17 |issue=1 |pages=167–176 |doi=10.1093/icb/17.1.167 |url=https://academic.oup.com/icb/article-pdf/17/1/167/410759/17-1-167.pdf |jstor=3882252 }}

{{cite journal |last1=Houck |first1=Lynne D. |title=Pheromone communication in amphibians and reptiles |journal=Annual Review of Physiology |date=2009 |volume=71 |issue=1 |pages=161–176 |doi=10.1146/annurev.physiol.010908.163134 |pmid=18847365}}

{{cite journal |last1=López |first1=Pilar |last2=Aragón |first2=Pedro |last3=Martin |first3=José |title=Iberian rock lizards (Lacerta monticola cyreni) assess conspecific information using composite signals from faecal pellets |journal=Ethology |date=2010 |volume=104 |issue=10 |pages=809–820 |doi=10.1111/j.1439-0310.1998.tb00033.x}}

{{cite book |chapter=Communication and social behavior |title=Herpetology: An Introductory Biology of Amphibians and Reptiles |edition=4th |first1=Laurie J. |last1=Vitt |first2=Janalee P. |last2=Caldwell |name-list-style=amp |publisher=Academic Press |year=2014 |pages=255–289 |oclc=839312807}}

{{cite book |chapter=Squamates—Part I. Lizards |title=Herpetology: An Introductory Biology of Amphibians and Reptiles |edition=4th |first1=Laurie J. |last1=Vitt |first2=Janalee P. |last2=Caldwell |name-list-style=amp |publisher=Academic Press |year=2014 |pages=555–596 |oclc=839312807}}

}}

{{Animal communication}}

Category:Animal communication

Communication

Category:Articles containing video clips