Matthew effect
{{short description|The rich get richer and the poor get poorer}}
File:King James Bible 1772 - Matthew 25 (2) (cropped).jpg]]
The Matthew effect, sometimes called the Matthew principle or cumulative advantage,{{cite journal |last1=Merton |first1=Robert K. |title=The Matthew Effect in Science, II: Cumulative Advantage and the Symbolism of Intellectual Property |journal=Isis |date=1 December 1988 |volume=79 |issue=4 |pages=606–623 |doi=10.1086/354848|s2cid=17167736 }} is the tendency of individuals to accrue social or economic success in proportion to their initial level of popularity, friends, and wealth. It is sometimes summarized by the adage or platitude "the rich get richer and the poor get poorer".{{Cite book| edition = 1| publisher = Little, Brown and Company| isbn = 978-0-316-01792-3| last = Gladwell| first = Malcolm| title = Outliers: The Story of Success| date = 2008-11-18| url-access = registration| url = https://archive.org/details/outliersstoryofs00glad}}{{cite web |access-date=2009-01-12 |url=https://www.wsj.com/articles/SB122671469296530435 |title=The Elements of Success |work=The Wall Street Journal |date=2008-11-15 |author=Shaywitz, David A. }} Also termed the "Matthew effect of accumulated advantage", taking its name from the Parable of the Talents in the biblical Gospel of Matthew, it was coined by sociologists Robert K. Merton and Harriet Zuckerman in 1968.
Early studies of Matthew effects were primarily concerned with the inequality in the way scientists were recognized for their work. However, Norman W. Storer, of Columbia University, led a new wave of research. He believed he discovered that the inequality that existed in the social sciences also existed in other institutions.Rigney, Daniel (2010). "Matthew Effects in the Economy.” The Matthew Effect: How Advantage Begets Further Advantage. Columbia University Press. pp. pp. 35–52.
Later, in network science, a form of the Matthew effect was discovered in internet networks and called preferential attachment. The mathematics used for this network analysis of the internet was later reapplied to the Matthew effect in general, whereby wealth or credit is distributed among individuals according to how much they already have. This has the net effect of making it increasingly difficult for low ranked individuals to increase their totals because they have fewer resources to risk over time, and increasingly easy for high rank individuals to preserve a large total because they have a large amount to risk.{{Cite journal|last=Perc|first=Matjaž|year=2014|title=The Matthew effect in empirical data|journal= Journal of the Royal Society Interface|volume=12|issue=104|page=20140378|doi=10.1098/rsif.2014.0378|pmc=4233686|pmid=24990288|arxiv=1408.5124|bibcode=2014arXiv1408.5124P}}
Etymology
The concept is named according to two of the parables of Jesus in the synoptic Gospels (Table 2, of the Eusebian Canons). The concept concludes both synoptic versions of the parable of the talents:
{{blockquote|For to every one who has will more be given, and he will have abundance; but from him who has not, even what he has will be taken away.|Matthew 25:29, RSV.|source=}}
{{blockquote|I tell you, that to every one who has will more be given; but from him who has not, even what he has will be taken away.|Luke 19:26, RSV.|source=}}
The concept concludes two of the three synoptic versions of the parable of the lamp under a bushel (absent in the version of Matthew):
{{blockquote|For to him who has will more be given; and from him who has not, even what he has will be taken away.|Mark 4:25, RSV.|source=}}
{{blockquote|Take heed then how you hear; for to him who has will more be given, and from him who has not, even what he thinks that he has will be taken away.|Luke 8:18, RSV.|source=}}
The concept is presented again in Matthew outside of a parable during Christ's explanation to his disciples of the purpose of parables:
{{blockquote|And he answered them, "To you it has been given to know the secrets of the kingdom of heaven, but to them it has not been given. For to him who has will more be given, and he will have abundance; but from him who has not, even what he has will be taken away."|Matthew 13:11–12, RSV.|source=}}
Yule process
Prior to being called "The Matthew effect", Udny Yule, in 1925, noticed the effect in flower populations, which in population growth studies is called the Yule process in his honor.
==Sociology of science==
=Cumulative advantage=
In the sociology of science, the first description of the Matthew effect was given by Price in 1976.{{cite journal | last=Price | first=D. J. de S. | title=A general theory of bibliometric and other cumulative advantage processes | journal=J. Amer. Soc. Inform. Sci. | volume=27 | pages=292–306 | year=1976 | url=http://garfield.library.upenn.edu/price/pricetheory1976.pdf | doi=10.1002/asi.4630270505 | issue=5 | access-date=2008-07-19 | archive-date=2020-12-01 | archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20201201023139/http://garfield.library.upenn.edu/price/pricetheory1976.pdf | url-status=live }} (He referred to the process as a "cumulative advantage" process.) His was also the first application of the process to the growth of a network, producing what would now be called a scale-free network. It is in the context of network growth that the process is most frequently studied today. Price also promoted preferential attachment as a possible explanation for power laws in many other phenomena, including Lotka's law of scientific productivity and Bradford's law of journal use.
=Coining the "Matthew effect"=
"Matthew effect" was a term coined by Robert K. Merton and Harriet Anne Zuckerman to describe how, among other things, eminent scientists will often get more credit than a comparatively unknown researcher, even if their work is similar; it also means that credit will usually be given to researchers who are already famous.{{cite journal |author=Merton, Robert K. |year=1968 |url=http://www.garfield.library.upenn.edu/merton/matthew1.pdf |title=The Matthew Effect in Science |journal=Science |volume=159 |issue=3810 |pages=56–63 |doi=10.1126/science.159.3810.56 |pmid=17737466|bibcode=1968Sci...159...56M |s2cid=3526819 }}{{cite journal | last1 = Merton | first1 = Robert K | year = 1988 | title = The Matthew Effect in Science, II: Cumulative advantage and the symbolism of intellectual property | url = http://garfield.library.upenn.edu/merton/matthewii.pdf| journal = Isis | volume = 79 | issue = 4| pages = 606–623 | doi=10.1086/354848| s2cid = 17167736 }} For example, a prize will almost always be awarded to the most senior researcher involved in a project, even if all the work was done by a graduate student. This was later formulated by Stephen Stigler as Stigler's law of eponymy – "No scientific discovery is named after its original discoverer" – with Stigler explicitly naming Merton as the true discoverer, making his "law" an example of itself. Merton and Zuckerman further argued that in the scientific community the Matthew effect reaches beyond simple reputation to influence the wider communication system, playing a part in social selection processes and resulting in a concentration of resources and talent. They gave as an example the disproportionate visibility given to articles from acknowledged authors, at the expense of equally valid or superior articles written by unknown authors. They also noted that the concentration of attention on eminent individuals can lead to an increase in their self-assurance, pushing them to perform research in important but risky problem areas.
The Matthew Effect also relates to broader patterns of scientific productivity, which can be explained by additional sociological concepts in science, such as the sacred spark, cumulative advantage, and search costs minimization by journal editors. The sacred spark paradigm suggests that scientists differ in their initial abilities, talent, skills, persistence, work habits, etc. that provide particular individuals with an early advantage. These factors have a multiplicative effect which helps these scholars succeed later. The cumulative advantage model argues that an initial success helps a researcher gain access to resources (e.g., teaching release, best graduate students, funding, facilities, etc.), which in turn results in further success. Search costs minimization by journal editors takes place when editors try to save time and effort by consciously or subconsciously selecting articles from well-known scholars. Whereas the exact mechanism underlying these phenomena is yet unknown, it is documented that a minority of all academics produce the most research output and attract the most citations.{{cite journal |last1=Serenko |first1=A. |last2=Cox |first2=R. |last3=Bontis |first3=N. |last4=Booker |first4=L. |year=2011 |title=The Superstar Phenomenon in the Knowledge Management and Intellectual Capital Academic Discipline |url=https://www.aserenko.com/papers/JOI_Serenko_Superstar_Phenomenon_Published.pdf |journal=Journal of Informetrics |volume=5 |pages=333–345}}
In addition to its influence on recognition and productivity, the Matthew Effect can also be observed in the distribution of scientific resources, such as funding. A large Matthew effect was discovered in a study of science funding in the Netherlands, where winners just above the funding threshold were found to accumulate more than twice as much funding during the subsequent eight years as non-winners with near-identical review scores that fell just below the threshold.{{cite journal |last1=Bol |first1=T. |last2=de Vaan |first2=M. |last3=van de Rijt |first3=A. |year=2018 |title=The Matthew Effect in Science Funding |url=http://www.pnas.org/content/pnas/115/19/4887.full.pdf |journal=PNAS |volume=115 |issue=19 |pages=4887–4890 |bibcode=2018PNAS..115.4887B |doi=10.1073/pnas.1719557115 |pmc=5948972 |pmid=29686094 |doi-access=free}}
Education
In education, the term "Matthew effect" has been adopted by psychologist Keith Stanovich{{cite journal | last=Stanovich | first=Keith E. | title=Matthew Effects in Reading: Some Consequences of Individual Differences in the Acquisition of Literacy | journal=Reading Research Quarterly | volume=21 | date=1986 | issue=1–2 | doi=10.1177/0022057409189001-204 | pages=360–407}} and popularised by education theorist Anthony Kelly to describe a phenomenon observed in research on how new readers acquire the skills to read. Effectively, early success in acquiring reading skills usually leads to later successes in reading as the learner grows, while failing to learn to read before the third or fourth year of schooling may be indicative of lifelong problems in learning new skills.{{Cite journal|last=Kempe, C., Eriksson-Gustavsson, A. L., & Samuelsson, S|year=2011|title=Are There any Matthew Effects in Literacy and Cognitive Development?|journal=Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research|volume=55|issue=2|pages=181–196|doi=10.1080/00313831.2011.554699|s2cid=145163197}}
This is because children who fall behind in reading would read less, increasing the gap between them and their peers. Later, when students need to "read to learn" (where before they were learning to read), their reading difficulty creates difficulty in most other subjects. In this way they fall further and further behind in school, dropping out at a much higher rate than their peers.Adams, Marilyn J. (1990). Beginning to Read: Thinking and Learning about Print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. pp. 59–60. This effect has been used in legal cases, such as Brody v. Dare County Board of Education.{{Cite web |title=Wrightslaw - North Carolina, Review Officer Special Education Decision |url=https://www.wrightslaw.com/law/caselaw/case_Brody_RO_decision.html |access-date=2022-12-22 |website=www.wrightslaw.com}} Such cases argue that early education intervention is essential for disabled children, and that failing to do so negatively impacts those children.{{Cite web |title=Assessment & Testing - The Matthew Effect - Wrightslaw.com |url=https://www.wrightslaw.com/info/test.matthew.effect.htm |access-date=2022-12-22 |website=www.wrightslaw.com}}
A 2014 review of Matthew effect in education found mixed empirical evidence, where Matthew effect tends to describe the development of primary school skills, while a compensatory pattern was found for skills with ceiling effects.{{cite journal | last1=Pfost | first1=Maximilian | last2=Hattie | first2=John | last3=Dörfler | first3=Tobias | last4=Artelt | first4=Cordula | title=Individual Differences in Reading Development: A Review of 25 Years of Empirical Research on Matthew Effects in Reading | journal=Review of Educational Research | volume=84 | issue=2 | date=2014 | issn=0034-6543 | doi=10.3102/0034654313509492 | pages=203–244}} A 2016 study on reading comprehension assessments for 99 thousand students found a pattern of stable differences, with some narrowing of the gap for students with learning disabilities.{{cite journal | last1=Schulte | first1=Ann C. | last2=Stevens | first2=Joseph J. | last3=Elliott | first3=Stephen N. | last4=Tindal | first4=Gerald | last5=Nese | first5=Joseph F. T. | title=Achievement gaps for students with disabilities: Stable, widening, or narrowing on a state-wide reading comprehension test? | journal=Journal of Educational Psychology | volume=108 | issue=7 | date=2016 | issn=1939-2176 | doi=10.1037/edu0000107 | doi-access=free | pages=925–942}}
Network science
In network science, the Matthew effect was noticed as preferential attachment of earlier nodes in a network, which explains that these nodes tend to attract more links early on.{{Cite journal|last=Barabási|first=A-L|year=1999|title=Emergence of scaling in random networks|journal=Science|volume=286|issue=5439|pages=509–512|doi=10.1126/science.286.5439.509|pmid=10521342|author2=Albert, R|arxiv=cond-mat/9910332|bibcode=1999Sci...286..509B|s2cid=524106}}
The application of preferential attachment to the growth of the World Wide Web was proposed by Barabási and Albert in 1999.{{cite journal | last=Barabási | first=A.-L. |author2=R. Albert | title=Emergence of scaling in random networks | journal=Science | volume=286 | pages=509–512 | year=1999 | arxiv=cond-mat/9910332 | doi=10.1126/science.286.5439.509 | issue=5439 | pmid=10521342| bibcode=1999Sci...286..509B | s2cid=524106 }} Barabási and Albert also coined the name "preferential attachment", and suggested that the process might apply to the growth of other networks as well. For growing networks, the precise functional form of preferential attachment can be estimated by maximum likelihood estimation.{{cite journal |last1=Pham |first1=Thong |last2=Sheridan |first2=Paul |last3=Shimodaira |first3=Hidetoshi |title=PAFit: A Statistical Method for Measuring Preferential Attachment in Temporal Complex Networks |journal=PLOS ONE |date=September 17, 2015 |volume=10 |issue=9 |doi=10.1371/journal.pone.0137796 |pages=e0137796 |pmid=26378457 |pmc=4574777|bibcode=2015PLoSO..1037796P |doi-access=free }}
Due to preferential attachment, Matjaž Perc writes "a node that acquires more connections than another one will increase its connectivity at a higher rate, and thus an initial difference in the connectivity between two nodes will increase further as the network grows, while the degree of individual nodes will grow proportional with the square root of time." The Matthew Effect therefore explains the growth of some nodes in vast networks such as the Internet.{{Cite book|title=Networks, Complexity And Internet Regulation – Scale-Free Law|last=Guadamuz|first=Andres|date=2011|publisher=Edward Elgar|isbn=9781848443105}}
Career progression
A model for career progress quantitatively incorporates the Matthew Effect in order to predict the distribution of individual career length in competitive professions. The model predictions are validated by analyzing the empirical distributions of career length for careers in science and professional sports (e.g. Major League Baseball).{{Cite journal |last=Petersen |first=Alexander M. |author2=Jung, Woo-Sung |author3=Yang, Jae-Suk |author4=Stanley, H. Eugene |year=2011 |title=Quantitative and Empirical demonstration of the Matthew Effect in a study of Career Longevity |journal=PNAS |volume=108 |issue=1 |pages=18–23 |arxiv=0806.1224 |bibcode=2011PNAS..108...18P |doi=10.1073/pnas.1016733108 |pmc=3017158 |pmid=21173276 |doi-access=free}} As a result, the disparity between the large number of short careers and the relatively small number of extremely long careers can be explained by the "rich-get-richer" mechanism, which in this framework, provides more experienced and more reputable individuals with a competitive advantage in obtaining new career opportunities.
Bask (2024) reviewed theoretical research on academic career progression and found that Feichtinger et al. developed a model where a researcher’s reputation grows through scientific effort but declines without continual activity{{Cite journal |last=Bask |first=Mikael |date=2024-12-01 |title=Skill, status and the Matthew effect: a theoretical framework |journal=Journal of Computational Social Science |language=en |volume=7 |issue=3 |pages=2221–2253 |doi=10.1007/s42001-024-00298-z |issn=2432-2725|doi-access=free }}. Their model incorporates the Matthew effect, in that researchers with high initial reputations benefit more from their efforts, while those with low reputations may see theirs diminish even with similar effort. They showed that if a researcher starts with low reputation, their career is likely to decline and eventually end, whereas researchers starting with high reputation may either sustain a successful career or face early exit depending on their effort over time.
Markets with social influence
Experiments manipulating download counts or bestseller lists for books and music have shown consumer activity follows the apparent popularity.{{cite journal |last1=Salganik |first1=Matthew J. |author-link=Salganik, Matthew J. |last2=Dodds |first2=Peter S. |last3=Watts |first3=Duncan J. |year=2006 |title=Experimental Study of Inequality and Unpredictability in an Artificial Cultural Market |url=http://www.princeton.edu/~mjs3/salganik_dodds_watts06_full.pdf |journal=Science |volume=311 |issue=5762 |pages=854–856 |bibcode=2006Sci...311..854S |doi=10.1126/science.1121066 |pmid=16469928 |s2cid=7310490}}{{cite journal |last1=Sorenson |first1=Alan T |year=2007 |title=Bestseller Lists and Product Variety |url=http://www.ssc.wisc.edu/~sorensen/papers/sorensen_JIE_2007.pdf |journal=Journal of Industrial Economics |volume=55 |issue=4 |pages=715–738 |doi=10.1111/j.1467-6451.2007.00327.x |s2cid=49028945}}{{cite journal |last1=van de Rijt |first1=A. |last2=Kang |first2=S. |last3=Restivo |first3=M. |last4=Patil |first4=A. |year=2014 |title=Field Experiments of Success-Breeds-Success Dynamics |url=http://www.pnas.org/content/pnas/111/19/6934.full.pdf |journal=PNAS |volume=111 |issue=19 |pages=6934–6939 |bibcode=2014PNAS..111.6934V |doi=10.1073/pnas.1316836111 |pmc=4024896 |pmid=24778230 |doi-access=free}}
Social influence often induces a rich-get-richer phenomenon where popular products tend to become even more popular.
{{cite journal
| title= Transient dynamics in trial-offer markets with social influence: Trade-offs between appeal and quality
| author1= Altszyler, E |author2= Berbeglia, F.
| author3= Berbeglia, G. |author4= Van Hentenryck, P.
| journal= PLOS ONE | year= 2017 |volume= 12 |issue= 7
| doi= 10.1371/journal.pone.0180040
|pmid = 28746334| page= e0180040 |bibcode= 2017PLoSO..1280040A
| pmc= 5528888| doi-access= free }}
An example of the Matthew Effect's role on social influence is an experiment by Salganik, Dodds, and Watts in which they created an experimental virtual market named MUSICLAB. In MUSICLAB, people could listen to music and choose to download the songs they enjoyed the most. The song choices were unknown songs produced by unknown bands. There were two groups tested; one group was given zero additional information on the songs and one group was told the popularity of each song and the number of times it had previously been downloaded.
{{cite conference
| url= https://ojs.aaai.org/index.php/AAAI/article/view/10511/10370
| format= PDF
| title= Taming the Matthew Effect in Online Markets with Social Influence
| author1= Berbeglia, F.
| author2= Van Hentenryck, P.
| conference= Thirty-First AAAI Conference on Artificial Intelligence
| volume= 31
| date= 2017-02-10
| location= San Francisco
| access-date= 2022-12-30
| url-status= live
| archive-url= https://web.archive.org/web/20221230005902/https://ojs.aaai.org/index.php/AAAI/article/download/10511/10370
| archive-date= 2022-12-30
| doi= 10.1609/aaai.v31i1.10511
| doi-access= free
}}
As a result, the group that saw which songs were the most popular and were downloaded the most were then biased to choose those songs as well. The songs that were most popular and downloaded the most stayed at the top of the list and consistently received the most plays. To summarize the experiment's findings, the performance rankings had the largest effect boosting expected downloads the most. Download rankings had a decent effect; however, not as impactful as the performance rankings.
{{cite journal
| last1= Salganik
| first1= Matthew J.
| last2= Dodds
| first2= Peter S.
| last3= Watts
| first3= Duncan J.
| date= 2006-02-10
| title= Experimental Study of Inequality and Unpredictability in an Artificial Cultural Market
| url= https://www.science.org/doi/10.1126/science.1121066
| url-access= subscription
| journal= Science
| volume= 311
| issue= 5762
| pages= 854–856
| doi= 10.1126/science.1121066
| pmid=16469928 | bibcode=2006Sci...311..854S | s2cid=7310490 }}
Abeliuk et al. (2016) also proved that when utilizing "performance rankings", a monopoly will be created for the most popular songs.
{{cite journal
| last1= Abeliuk
| first1= Andrés
| last2= Berbeglia
| first2= Gerardo
| last3= Cebrian
| first3= Manuel
| last4= Van Hentenryck
| first4= Pascal
| editor-last= Huerta-Quintanilla
| editor-first= Rodrigo
| date= 2015-04-01
| title= The Benefits of Social Influence in Optimized Cultural Markets
| journal= PLOS ONE
| volume= 10
| issue= 4
| pages=e0121934 | doi=10.1371/journal.pone.0121934
| pmid=25831093 | pmc=4382093 | bibcode=2015PLoSO..1021934A | doi-access= free
}}
Cumulative inequality theory
{{Original research section|date=April 2025}}
The ideas of this theory were developed by Kenneth Ferraro and colleagues as an integrative or middle-range theory. Originally specified in five axioms and nineteen propositions, cumulative inequality theory incorporates elements from the following theories and perspectives, several of which are related to the study of society:
- Robert Merton articulated the Matthew effect to explain accumulating advantage
- Glen Elder's life course perspective
- Stress process theory
- Age stratification theory.
In recent years, Ferraro and several other researchers have been testing and elaborating elements of the theory on a variety of topics to provide evidence for the theoretical framework. In the following information you will find some of the uses of this theory in sociological studies. '"social systems generate inequality, which is manifested over the life course via demographic and developmental processes."{{cite journal |last1=Ferraro |first1=K. F. |last2=Shippee |first2=T. P. |title=Aging and Cumulative Inequality: How Does Inequality Get Under the Skin? |journal=The Gerontologist |date=17 April 2009 |volume=49 |issue=3 |pages=333–343 |doi=10.1093/geront/gnp034 |pmid=19377044 |pmc=2721665 }}
McDonough, Worts, Booker, et al. (2015) for example studied cumulative disadvantage in the generations of health inequality among mothers in Britain and the United States. The study examined "if adverse circumstances early in the life course cumulate as health harming biographical patterns across working and family caregiving years."{{cite journal |last1=McDonough |first1=Peggy |last2=Worts |first2=Diana |last3=Booker |first3=Cara |last4=McMunn |first4=Anne |last5=Sacker |first5=Amanda |title=Cumulative disadvantage, employment–marriage, and health inequalities among American and British mothers |journal=Advances in Life Course Research |date=September 2015 |volume=25 |pages=49–66 |doi=10.1016/j.alcr.2015.05.004 |doi-access=free }} Also, it was examined if institutional context moderated cumulative effects of micro level processes. The results showed that existing health disparities of women in midlife, during work and family rearing time, were intensified by cumulative disadvantages caused by adversities in early life. Thus, the accumulation of disadvantage had negative connotations for the well-being of women's occupational experiences and family life.
McLean (2010), on the other hand, studied U.S. combat and non combat veterans through cumulative disadvantage. He discovered that cumulated negative disadvantages caused by disability and unemployment were more likely to influence the lives of combat veterans versus non combat veterans. Combat veterans suffered physical and emotional trauma that had a disabling effect which impeded their ability to successfully obtain employment. . The research is crucial for social policy implementation that assist United States Veterans to find and retain employment that is suitable to their personal conditions.{{citation needed|date=March 2020}}
In continuation, Woolredge, Frank, Coulette, et al. (2016) studied the prison sentencing of racial groups. specifically of African American males with prior felony convictions. They examined how pre-trial processes affect trial outcomes. It was determined that cumulative disadvantage was existent for African American males and young men; the results were measured by: set bail amounts, pre-trial detention, prison sentencing, and no reduction in sentencing length. The research are striving to create changes in the justice system that reduce incarceration rates of African American Males by reducing bail amounts, and pre trial imprisonment. Further studies are important to decrease the incarceration of minority groups in society, and to create a non biased justice system.{{citation needed|date=March 2020}}
Additionally, Ferraro & Moore (2003) have applied the theory to the study of long-term consequences of early obesity for midlife health and socioeconomic attainment. The study shows that obesity experienced in early life leads to lower-body disability, but higher risk factors to health.{{cite journal |last1=Ferraro |first1=Kenneth F. |last2=Kelley-Moore |first2=Jessica A. |title=Cumulative Disadvantage and Health: Long-Term Consequences of Obesity? |journal=American Sociological Review |date=October 2003 |volume=68 |issue=5 |pages=707–729 |pmc=3348542 |pmid=22581979 |doi=10.2307/1519759 |jstor=1519759 }} Moreover. The research mentions a risk that has been brought to attention in the past years; it ties being over weight to negative stigma (DeJong 1980),and has influenced fair labor market positioning{{cite journal |last1=Averett |first1=S. |last2=Korenman |first2=S. |title=Black-white differences in social and economic consequences of obesity |journal=International Journal of Obesity |date=February 1999 |volume=23 |issue=2 |pages=166–173 |doi=10.1038/sj.ijo.0800805 |pmid=10078852 |doi-access=free }} and wages.{{cite journal |last1=Register |first1=Charles A |last2=Williams |first2=Donald R |title=Wage Effects of Obesity among Young Workers |journal=Social Science Quarterly |volume=71 |issue=1 |date=March 1, 1990 |pages=130–141 |url=https://www.proquest.com/openview/3a61ccb7d4d72a3e8761a11fd80e124a/1 }}
Lastly, Crystal, Shea, & Reyes (2016) studied the effects of cumulative advantage in increasing within age cohort economic inequalities in diverse periods of time. The study utilized economic patterns such as annual wealth value and household size. The inequalities of age were analyzed by using the gini coefficient. The study took place between 1980 and 2010. The results showed that at age 65 plus individuals had higher rates of inequality and it increased significantly for baby boomers or during economic recession and times of war. The research is written to estimate the possible impacts of social security changes on older adults in American Society.
In conclusion, Cumulative Inequality or Cumulative Disadvantage Theory, is broadly examining various topics that impact public policy, and the view of our role within society. Further benefits of the theory are still to be seen in the next coming years.
Life course inequality
The concept of cumulative advantage, based on Merton and Zuckerman's Matthew Effect, has been widely applied to the study of life course inequality.{{Cite journal |last=Dannefer |first=Dale |date=2003-11-01 |title=Cumulative Advantage/Disadvantage and the Life Course: Cross-Fertilizing Age and Social Science Theory |url=https://academic.oup.com/psychsocgerontology/article-abstract/58/6/S327/597436?redirectedFrom=fulltext |journal=The Journals of Gerontology: Series B |volume=58 |issue=6 |pages=S327–S337 |doi=10.1093/geronb/58.6.S327 |pmid=14614120 |issn=1079-5014}}{{Cite journal |last1=DiPrete |first1=Thomas A. |last2=Eirich |first2=Gregory M. |date=2006 |title=Cumulative Advantage as a Mechanism for Inequality: A Review of Theoretical and Empirical Developments |url=http://www.jstor.org/stable/29737740 |journal=Annual Review of Sociology |volume=32 |pages=271–297 |doi=10.1146/annurev.soc.32.061604.123127 |jstor=29737740 |issn=0360-0572}} Dannefer (2003) argued that inequalities in resources, health, and social status systematically widen over time, shaped by social institutions, economic structures, and psychosocial factors like perceived agency and self-efficacy. Early advantages or disadvantages become amplified, producing growing disparities as individuals age. Pallas (2009) further highlighted how cumulative advantage involves shifts between different types of capital, such as human, economic, and symbolic, complicating efforts to measure inequality over time.{{Cite journal |last1=Pallas |first1=Aaron M. |last2=Jennings |first2=Jennifer L. |date=2009 |title=Cumulative knowledge about cumulative advantage |url=https://www.e-periodica.ch/digbib/view?pid=soz-001:2009:35::675 |journal=Schweizerische Zeitschrift für Soziologie = Revue suisse de sociologie = Swiss journal of sociology |volume=35 |issue=2 |pages=211 |doi=10.5169/seals-815047 |issn=0379-3664}}
Research has expanded cumulative advantage beyond aging to domains such as education, work, health, and wealth. In education, early academic differences lead to greater access to opportunities and resources, compounding over time. In the workforce, initial job placements and early career achievements create divergent paths in earnings and occupational mobility. Family background and neighborhood contexts also play a role, reinforcing early disparities across the life course
Mitigation
Open Science is "the movement to make scientific research (including publications, data, physical samples, and software) and its dissemination accessible to all levels of society, amateur or professional". One of its key motivations is increasing equity in scientific endeavors. However, Ross-Hellauer, T. et. al. (2022) argue that Open Science's ambition to reduce inequalities in academia may inadvertently perpetuate or exacerbate existing disparities caused by cumulative advantage.{{Cite journal |last1=Ross-Hellauer |first1=Tony |last2=Reichmann |first2=Stefan |last3=Cole |first3=Nicki Lisa |last4=Fessl |first4=Angela |last5=Klebel |first5=Thomas |last6=Pontika |first6=Nancy |date=2022-01-19 |title=Dynamics of cumulative advantage and threats to equity in open science: a scoping review |journal=Royal Society Open Science |volume=9 |issue=1 |pages=211032 |doi=10.1098/rsos.211032 |pmc=8767192 |pmid=35116143|bibcode=2022RSOS....911032R }} As Open Science progresses, it faces the challenge of balancing its goals of openness and accessibility with the risk that its practices could reinforce the privileges of the more advantaged, particularly in terms of access to knowledge, technology, and funding. The authors make this critique to urge professionals to reflect "upon the ways in which implementation may run counter to ideals".
See also
{{cols|colwidth=14em}}
- Ability grouping
- Attention inequality
- Capital accumulation
- Convergence
- Google Scholar effect
- The internal contradictions of capital accumulation
- Lindy effect
- Matilda effect
- Metcalfe's law
- Pareto distribution
- Positive feedback
- Preferential attachment
- {{section link|Quotation|Misquotations}}
- Social network analysis
- Tracking (education)
- Virtuous circle and vicious circle
- Wealth concentration
{{colend}}
References
{{reflist|25em}}
Further reading
{{Refbegin|30em}}
- {{cite journal | last1 = Bahr | first1 = Peter Riley | year = 2007 | title = Double jeopardy: Testing the effects of multiple basic skill deficiencies on successful remediation | journal = Research in Higher Education | volume = 48 | issue = 6| pages = 695–725 | doi=10.1007/s11162-006-9047-y| s2cid = 144937969 }}
- {{cite journal |last1=Clarke |first1=Philippa |last2=Morenoff |first2=Jeffrey |last3=Debbink |first3=Michelle |last4=Golberstein |first4=Ezra |last5=Elliott |first5=Michael R. |last6=Lantz |first6=Paula M. |title=Cumulative Exposure to Neighborhood Context |journal=Research on Aging |date=2 January 2013 |volume=36 |issue=1 |pages=115–142 |doi=10.1177/0164027512470702 |pmid=24465068 |pmc=3900407 }}
- {{cite journal |last1=Clarke |first1=P. J. |last2=O'Malley |first2=P. M. |last3=Schulenberg |first3=J. E. |last4=Johnston |first4=L. D. |title=Midlife Health and Socioeconomic Consequences of Persistent Overweight Across Early Adulthood: Findings From a National Survey of American Adults (1986-2008) |journal=American Journal of Epidemiology |date=7 July 2010 |volume=172 |issue=5 |pages=540–548 |doi=10.1093/aje/kwq156 |pmid=20610468 |pmc=2950821 }}
- Cunningham, A. E., & Chen, Y.–J. (2014). Rich-get-richer effect (Matthew Effects). In P. Brooks & V. Kempe (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Language Development. New York: Sage.
- {{cite journal |last1=Dannefer |first1=D. |title=Cumulative Advantage/Disadvantage and the Life Course: Cross-Fertilizing Age and Social Science Theory |journal=The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences |date=1 November 2003 |volume=58 |issue=6 |pages=S327–S337 |doi=10.1093/geronb/58.6.S327 |pmid=14614120 |doi-access=free }}
- {{cite journal |last1=DeJong |first1=William |title=The Stigma of Obesity: The Consequences of Naive Assumptions Concerning the Causes of Physical Deviance |journal=Journal of Health and Social Behavior |date=1980 |volume=21 |issue=1 |pages=75–87 |doi=10.2307/2136696 |pmid=7365232 |jstor=2136696 }}
- {{cite book |last1=Elder |first1=G.H. |year=1974 |title=Children of the Great Depression: Social change in life experience |location=Chicago |publisher=University of Chicago Press }}
- {{cite journal |last1=Elder |first1=Glen H. |title=Time, Human Agency, and Social Change: Perspectives on the Life Course |journal=Social Psychology Quarterly |date=1994 |volume=57 |issue=1 |pages=4–15 |doi=10.2307/2786971 |jstor=2786971 }}
- {{cite book |last1=Ferraro |first1=K.F. |last2=Shippee |first2=T.P. |last3=Schafer |first3=M.H. |year=2009 |chapter=Cumulative inequality theory for research on aging and the life course |chapter-url={{Google books|lQv7-xPm_jsC|page=413|plainurl=yes}} |editor1-first=V.L. |editor1-last=Bengtson |editor2-first=M. |editor2-last=Silverstein |editor3-first=N.M. |editor3-last=Putney |editor4-first=D. |editor4-last=Gans |title=Handbook of theories of aging |location=New York |publisher=Springer }}
- {{cite journal |last1=Goosby |first1=Bridget J. |title=Early Life Course Pathways of Adult Depression and Chronic Pain |journal=Journal of Health and Social Behavior |date=20 February 2013 |volume=54 |issue=1 |pages=75–91 |doi=10.1177/0022146512475089 |pmid=23426854 |pmc=3733784 }}
- {{cite journal |last1=McDade |first1=T. W. |last2=Lindau |first2=S. T. |last3=Wroblewski |first3=K. |title=Predictors of C-Reactive Protein in the National Social Life, Health, and Aging Project |journal=The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences |date=19 February 2010 |volume=66B |issue=1 |pages=129–136 |doi=10.1093/geronb/gbq008 |pmid=20172904 |pmc=3001750 }}
- {{cite journal |last1=MacLean |first1=Alair |title=The Things They Carry |journal=American Sociological Review |date=17 June 2010 |volume=75 |issue=4 |pages=563–585 |doi=10.1177/0003122410374085 |pmid=21818157 |pmc=3148716 }}
- {{cite journal |last1=Merton |first1=R. K. |title=The Matthew Effect in Science: The reward and communication systems of science are considered |journal=Science |date=5 January 1968 |volume=159 |issue=3810 |pages=56–63 |doi=10.1126/science.159.3810.56 |pmid=17737466 |bibcode=1968Sci...159...56M |s2cid=3526819 }}
- {{cite journal |last1=Merton |first1=Robert K. |title=The Matthew Effect in Science, II: Cumulative Advantage and the Symbolism of Intellectual Property |journal=Isis |date=December 1988 |volume=79 |issue=4 |pages=606–623 |doi=10.1086/354848 |s2cid=17167736 }}
- {{cite journal |last1=Morton |first1=Patricia M. |last2=Schafer |first2=Markus H. |last3=Ferraro |first3=Kenneth F. |title=Does Childhood Misfortune Increase Cancer Risk in Adulthood? |journal=Journal of Aging and Health |date=4 July 2012 |volume=24 |issue=6 |pages=948–984 |doi=10.1177/0898264312449184 |pmid=22764155 |pmc=6036632 }}
- {{cite journal |last1=Morton |first1=Patricia M. |last2=Mustillo |first2=Sarah A. |last3=Ferraro |first3=Kenneth F. |title=Does childhood misfortune raise the risk of acute myocardial infarction in adulthood? |journal=Social Science & Medicine |date=March 2014 |volume=104 |pages=133–141 |doi=10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.11.026 |pmid=24581071 |pmc=4010612 }}
- {{cite journal |last1=O'Rand |first1=A. M. |title=The Precious and the Precocious: Understanding Cumulative Disadvantage and Cumulative Advantage Over the Life Course |journal=The Gerontologist |date=1 April 1996 |volume=36 |issue=2 |pages=230–238 |doi=10.1093/geront/36.2.230 |pmid=8920094 |doi-access=free }}
- {{cite journal |last1=Pearlin |first1=Leonard I. |last2=Schieman |first2=Scott |last3=Fazio |first3=Elena M. |last4=Meersman |first4=Stephen C. |title=Stress, Health, and the Life Course: Some Conceptual Perspectives |journal=Journal of Health and Social Behavior |date=22 June 2016 |volume=46 |issue=2 |pages=205–219 |doi=10.1177/002214650504600206 |pmid=16028458 |s2cid=23796774 }}
- {{cite journal |last1=Riley |first1=Matilda White |title=On the Significance of Age in Sociology |journal=American Sociological Review |date=1987 |volume=52 |issue=1 |pages=1–14 |doi=10.2307/2095388 |jstor=2095388 }}
- Rigney, Daniel (2010). The Matthew Effect: How Advantage Begets Further Advantage. Columbia University Press.
- {{cite book |last1=Riley |first1=M. W. |last2=Johnson |first2=M. |last3=Foner |first3=A. |year=1972 |title=Aging and society: Vol. 3. A sociology of age stratification |location=New York |publisher=Russell Sage Foundation }}
- {{cite journal |last1=Schafer |first1=Markus H. |last2=Ferraro |first2=Kenneth F. |last3=Mustillo |first3=Sarah A. |title=Children of Misfortune: Early Adversity and Cumulative Inequality in Perceived Life Trajectories |journal=American Journal of Sociology |date=January 2011 |volume=116 |issue=4 |pages=1053–1091 |doi=10.1086/655760 |pmid=21648247 |pmc=3149822 }}
- {{cite journal |last1=Shippee |first1=Tetyana P. |last2=Rinaldo |first2=Lindsay |last3=Ferraro |first3=Kenneth F. |title=Mortality Risk Among Black and White Working Women |journal=Journal of Aging and Health |date=28 September 2011 |volume=24 |issue=1 |pages=141–167 |doi=10.1177/0898264311422743 |pmid=21956101 |pmc=3347922 }}
- {{cite journal |last1=Crystal |first1=Stephen |last2=Shea |first2=Dennis G. |last3=Reyes |first3=Adriana M. |title=Cumulative Advantage, Cumulative Disadvantage, and Evolving Patterns of Late-Life Inequality |journal=The Gerontologist |volume=57 |issue=5 |date=30 March 2016 |pages=910–920 |doi=10.1093/geront/gnw056 |pmid=27030008 |pmc=5881660 }}
- {{cite journal | last1 = Stanovich | first1 = Keith E | year = 1986 | title = Matthew Effects in Reading: Some Consequences of Individual Differences in the Acquisition of Literacy | url = http://www.psychologytoday.com/files/u81/Stanovich__1986_.pdf | journal = Reading Research Quarterly | volume = 21 | issue = 4| pages = 360–407 | doi=10.1598/rrq.21.4.1| doi-broken-date = 6 May 2025 }}
- Stanovich, Keith E. (2000). Progress in Understanding Reading: Scientific Foundations and New Frontiers. New York: Guilford Press.nningham, A. E., & Chen, Y.–J. (2014). Rich-get-richer effect (Matthew Effects). In P. Brooks & V. Kempe (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Language Development. New York: Sage.
- {{cite journal |last1=Stunkard |first1=A. J. |last2=LaFleur |first2=W. R. |last3=Wadden |first3=T. A. |title=Stigmatization of obesity in medieval times: Asia and Europe |journal=International Journal of Obesity |date=December 1998 |volume=22 |issue=12 |pages=1141–1144 |doi=10.1038/sj.ijo.0800753 |pmid=9877248 |doi-access=free }}
- {{cite journal |last1=Wooldredge |first1=John |last2=Frank |first2=James |last3=Goulette |first3=Natalie |last4=Travis |first4=Lawrence |title=Is the Impact of Cumulative Disadvantage on Sentencing Greater for Black Defendants? |journal=Criminology & Public Policy |date=May 2015 |volume=14 |issue=2 |pages=187–223 |doi=10.1111/1745-9133.12124 }}
{{refend}}
{{DEFAULTSORT:Matthew Effect (sociology)}}
Category:Sociological terminology