Religiosity

{{short description|Degree of religious commitment or involvement}}

{{Lead too short|date=October 2020}}

[[File:Importance of Religion in the World (Gallup Poll 2008-2009).svg|thumb|550px|Results of a 2008/2009 Gallup poll on whether respondents said that religion was "important in [their] daily life."{{cite web|last1=Crabtree|first1=Steve|title=Religiosity Highest in World's Poorest Nations|date=31 August 2010 |url=http://www.gallup.com/poll/142727/religiosity-highest-world-poorest-nations.aspx|publisher=Gallup|access-date=27 May 2015}} (in which numbers have been rounded)[https://worldview.gallup.com/default.aspx GALLUP WorldView] - data accessed on 17 January 2009

border="0" cellspacing="0" cellpadding="0" style="width:100%;"
valign="top"|

{{legend|#002255|90%-100%}}

{{legend|#0044aa|80%-89%}}

{{legend|#0055d4|70%-79%}}

{{legend|#0066ff|60%-69%}}

| valign=top |

{{legend|#2a7fff|50%-59%}}

{{legend|#5599ff|40%-49%}}

{{legend|#80b3ff|30%-39%}}

{{legend|#aaccff|20%-29%}}

| valign=top |

{{legend|#d5e5ff|10%-19%}}

{{legend|white|0%-9%}}

{{legend|#c0c0c0ff|No data}}

]]

The Oxford English Dictionary defines religiosity as: "Religiousness; religious feeling or belief. [...] Affected or excessive religiousness".{{oed | religiosity}}. The earliest recorded usage of the former meaning is from 1382 Wycliffe's Bible, and of the latter is from 1799 by William Taylor quoted in John Warden Robberds' 1843 Memoir. Different scholars have seen this concept as broadly about religious orientations and degrees of involvement or commitment. The contrast between "religious" and "{{linktext|religiose}}" (superficially religious) and the concept of "strengthening" faith

For example:

{{cite book

|last1 = Maclaren

|first1 = Alexander

|author-link1 = Alexander Maclaren

|editor-last1 = Nicoll

|editor-first1 = William Robertson

|editor-link1 = William Robertson Nicoll

|year = 1887

|chapter = Conciliatory and Hortatory Transition to Polemics

|title = The Expositor's Bible

|url = https://books.google.com/books?id=zwVFAQAAMAAJ

|volume = 42: Colossians and Philemon

|publication-place = London

|publisher = Hodder and Stoughton

|page = 182

|access-date = 11 July 2024

|quote = The faith which is already the firmest, and by its firmness may gladden an Apostle, is still capable of and needs strengthening.

}}

suggest differences in the intensity of religiosity.

Scholars attempt to measure religiosity at the levels of individuals or groups, but differ as to what behaviors constitute religiosity.{{cite journal |last1= Holdcroft|first1=Barbara|title= What is Religiosity? |journal= Catholic Education: A Journal of Inquiry and Practice|date= September 2006|volume= 10|issue= 1|pages=89–103}} Sociologists of religion have observed that an individual's experience, beliefs, sense of belonging, and general behavior often are not congruent with their religious behavior, since there is much diversity in how one can be religious or not. Problems arise in measuring religiosity. For instance, measures of variables such as church attendance produce different results when different methods are used, such as traditional surveys as opposed to time-use surveys.

{{cite journal

|last1=Rossi |first1=Maurizio

|last2=Scappini |first2=Ettore

|title=Church Attendance, Problems of Measurement, and Interpreting Indicators: A Study of Religious Practice in the United States, 1975-2010

|journal=Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion

|date=June 2014 |volume=53 |issue=2 |pages=249–267

|doi=10.1111/jssr.12115 |issn=0021-8294

|doi-access=free }}

Components

The measurement of religiosity is hampered by the difficulties involved in defining what is meant by the term and what components it includes. Numerous studies have explored the different components of religiosity, with most finding some distinction between religious beliefs/doctrine, religious practice, and spirituality. When religiosity is measured, it is important to specify which aspects of religiosity are being discussed.{{cite journal|last1=Holdcroft|first1=Barbara|title=What is Religiosity?|journal=Catholic Education: A Journal of Inquiry and Practice|date=September 2006|volume=10|issue=1|pages=89–103}}

Numerous studies have explored the different components of human religiosity.Brink, T.L. 1993. Religiosity: measurement. in Survey of Social Science: Psychology, Frank N. Magill, Ed., Pasadena, CA: Salem Press, 1993, pp. 2096–2102.Hill, Peter C. and Hood, Ralph W. Jr. 1999. Measures of Religiosity. Birmingham, Alabama: Religious Education Press. {{ISBN|0-89135-106-X}} What most have found (often using factor analysis) is that there are multiple dimensions. For instance, Marie Cornwall and colleagues identify six dimensions of religiosity based on the understanding that there are at least three components to religious behavior: knowing (cognition in the mind), feeling (effect to the spirit), and doing (behavior of the body).{{cite journal | last1 = Cornwall | first1 = Marie | last2 = Albrecht | first2 = S.L. | last3 = Cunningham | first3 = P.H. | last4 = Pitcher | first4 = B.L. | year = 1986 | title = The dimensions of religiosity: A conceptual model with an empirical test | journal = Review of Religious Research | volume = 27 | issue = 3 | pages = 226–244 | doi = 10.2307/3511418 | jstor = 3511418 }} For each of these components of religiosity, there were two cross classifications, resulting in the six dimensions:

  • Cognition
  • traditional orthodoxy
  • particularistic orthodoxy
  • Affect
  • Palpable
  • Tangible
  • Behavior
  • religious behavior
  • religious participation

Sociologists have differed over the exact number of components of religiosity. Charles Glock's five-dimensional approach (Glock, 1972: 39) was among the first of its kind in the field of sociology of religion.Glock, C. Y. (1972). "On the Study of Religious Commitment" in J. E. Faulkner (ed.) Religion's Influence in Contemporary Society: Readings in the Sociology of Religion, Ohio: Charles E. Merril: 38–56. Other sociologists adapted Glock's list to include additional components (see for example, a six component measure by Mervin F. Verbit).{{cite journal | last1 = Verbit | first1 = M. F. | year = 1970 | title = The components and dimensions of religious behavior: Toward a reconceptualization of religiosity | journal = American Mosaic | volume = 24 | page = 39 }}{{cite journal|last1=Küçükcan|first1=T|year=2005|title=Multidimensional Approach to Religion: a way of looking at religious phenomena|journal=Journal for the Study of Religions and Ideologies|volume=4|issue=10|pages=60–70}}Küçükcan, Talip. 2000. [http://www.eskieserler.com/dosyalar/mpdf%20(1135).pdf Can Religiosity Be Measured? Dimensions of Religious Commitment: Theories Revisited]. Other researchers have found different dimensions, generally ranging from four to twelve components.

What most measures of religiosity find is that there is at least some distinction between religious doctrine, religious practice, and spirituality. Most dimensions of religiosity are correlated, meaning people who often attend church services (practice dimension) are also likely to score highly on the belief and spirituality dimensions. Nonetheless, an individual's scores on a measure of religiosity can vary between dimensions; they may not score high on all dimensions or low on all dimensions.

For example{{original research inline|date=February 2011}}, an individual could accept truthfulness of the Bible (belief dimension), but never attend a church or even belong to an organized religion (practice dimension). Another example is an individual who did not accept orthodox Christian doctrines (belief dimension) but did attend a charismatic worship service (practice dimension) in order to develop his/her sense of oneness with the divine (spirituality dimension). A different individual might disavow all doctrines associated with organized religions (belief dimension), not affiliate with an organized religion or attend religious services (practice dimension), and at the same time be strongly committed to a higher power and feel that the connection with that higher power is ultimately relevant (spirituality dimension). These are explanatory examples of the broadest dimensions of religiosity and may not be reflected in specific religiosity measures.

Demographic studies often show a wide diversity of religious beliefs, belonging, and practices in both religious and non-religious populations. For instance, among Americans who are not religious and not seeking religion, 68% believe in God, 12% are atheists, 17% are agnostics. Also, 18%{{who|date=June 2024|reason=This appears to still be referring to Americans, but which group of Americans, or all?}} self-identify as religious, 37% self-identify as spiritual but not religious, and 42% self-identify as neither spiritual nor religious. Furthermore, 21%{{who|date=June 2024|reason=This appears to still be referring to Americans, but which group of Americans, or all?}} pray every day and 24% pray once a month.{{cite web|url=http://commons.trincoll.edu/aris/files/2011/08/NONES_08.pdf|title=American Nones: The Profile of the No Religion Population|year=2008|publisher=American Religious Identification Survey|access-date=2014-01-30}}{{cite web |url=http://www.pewforum.org/2012/10/09/nones-on-the-rise-religion/ |title=Religion and the Unaffiliated |work="Nones" on the Rise |date=October 9, 2012 |publisher=Pew Research Center: Religion & Public Life }}{{cite web|title=Most of the Religiously Unaffiliated Still Keep Belief in God|url=http://www.pewresearch.org/daily-number/most-of-the-religiously-unaffiliated-still-keep-belief-in-god/|website=Pew Research Center|date=November 15, 2012}} Global studies on religion also show diversity.{{cite web|title=The Global Religious Landscape|url=http://www.pewforum.org/2012/12/18/global-religious-landscape-exec/|website=Pew Research Center|date=2012-12-18}}

Decades of anthropological, sociological, and psychological research have established that the common assumption of "religious congruence" is rarely accurate. "Religious congruence" is the view that religious beliefs and values are tightly integrated in an individual's mind, or that religious practices and behaviors follow directly from religious beliefs, or that religious beliefs are chronologically linear and stable across different contexts. People's religious ideas are fragmented, loosely connected, and context-dependent, like their ideas in all other domains of culture and life. The beliefs, affiliations, and behaviors of any individual are complex activities that have many sources including culture. Mark Chaves gives the following examples of religious incongruence: "Observant Jews may not believe what they say in their Sabbath prayers. Christian ministers may not believe in God. And people who regularly dance for rain don't do it in the dry season."{{cite journal|last1=Chaves|first1=Mark|title=SSSR Presidential Address Rain Dances in the Dry Season: Overcoming the Religious Congruence Fallacy|journal=Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion|date=March 2010|volume=49|issue=1|pages=1–14|doi=10.1111/j.1468-5906.2009.01489.x}}

Difficulties in measurement

=Polls and surveys=

Decades of anthropological, sociological, and psychological research have shown that congruence between an individual's beliefs, attitudes, and behavior concerning religion and irreligion is rare.{{Cite web|url=https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1468-5906.2009.01489.x|title=SSSR Presidential Address Rain Dances in the Dry Season: Overcoming the Religious Congruence Fallacy|author=Mark Chaves|website=Wiley|publisher=Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion|doi=10.1111/j.1468-5906.2009.01489.x|access-date=9 December 2024}}

The reliability of any poll results, in general and specifically on religion, can be questioned due numerous factors such as:{{cite book|title=Inventing American Religion: Polls Surveys, and the Tenuous Quest for a Nation's Faith|last1=Wuthnow|first1=Robert|publisher=Oxford University Press|year=2015|isbn=9780190258900|chapter=8. Taking Stock}}

  • there have been very low response rates for polls since the 1990s
  • polls consistently fail to predict government election outcomes, which signifies that polls in general do not capture the actual views of the population
  • biases in wording or topic affect how people respond to polls
  • polls categorize people based on limited choices
  • polls often generalize broadly
  • polls have shallow or superficial choices, which complicate expressing people's complex religious beliefs and practices
  • interviewer and respondent fatigue is very common

Researchers also note that an estimated 20–40% of the population changes their self-reported religious affiliation/identity over time due to numerous factors and that usually it is their answers on surveys that change, not necessarily their religious practices or beliefs.{{cite journal |last1=Johnson |first1=Byron |last2=Stark |first2=Rodney |last3=Bradshaw |first3=Matt |last4=Levin |first4=Jeff |title=Are Religious "Nones" Really Not Religious?: Revisiting Glenn, Three Decades Later |journal=Interdisciplinary Journal of Research on Religion |date=2022 |volume=18 |issue=7}}

In general, polling numbers are difficult to interpret and should not be taken at face value, since people in different cultural contexts may interpret the same questions differently.Holifield, E. Brooks (2015). Why Are Americans So Religious? The Limitations of Market Explanations. Religion and the Marketplace in the United States. pp. 33–60. ISBN 9780199361809. "Such numbers cannot be taken at face value. They do not simply represent the world as it is but are self-representations. The difference between how Americans and citizens of other Western nations answer pollsters’ questions is first of all about how they think of themselves and how they want to be thought of in the context in which the question is asked. It means something different to say that one is “very religious” in Picayune, Mississippi, than it does in Oslo. Someone might have many reasons to answer yes to such a question, and it might be misleading to interpret the “yes” as having one simple meaning."

Responses to Gallup polls on religiosity vary based on how the question is worded. Since the early 2000s, Gallup has routinely asked about complex topics like belief in God using three different question wordings and they have consistently received three different percentages in the responses.{{cite web |last1=Saad |first1=Lydia |last2=Hrynowski |first2=Zach |title=How Many Americans Believe in God? |url=https://news.gallup.com/poll/268205/americans-believe-god.aspx |website=Gallup.com |publisher=Gallup |language=en |date=24 June 2022 |quote=The answer to how many Americans believe in God depends on how the question is asked. Gallup has measured U.S. adults' belief in God three different ways in recent years, with varying results.}}

== In the United States ==

Two major surveys in the United States, the General Social Survey (GSS) and the Cooperative Congressional Election Study (CCES), have consistently produced discrepancies between their demographic estimates on religion that amount to 8% and growing. This is due to a few factors, such as differences in question wording that impact participant responses due to "social desirability bias"; the lumping of very different groups (atheist, agnostics, nothing in particular) into singular categories (e.g., "no religion" vs "nothing in particular"); and differences in the representativeness of the samples (e.g., "nones" are more politically moderate in the GSS sample than in the CCES sample, while Protestants are more conservative in the CCES sample than in the GSS sample).{{cite journal |last1=Burge |first1=Ryan P. |title=How Many "Nones" Are There? Explaining the Discrepancies in Survey Estimates |journal=Review of Religious Research |date=March 2020 |volume=62 |issue=1 |pages=173–190 |doi=10.1007/s13644-020-00400-7|s2cid=256240351 }}

The 2008 American Religious Identification Survey (ARIS) found a difference between how people identify and what people believe. While only 0.7% of U.S. adults identified as atheist, 2.3% said there is no such thing as a god. Only 0.9% identified as agnostic, but 10.0% said there is either no way to know if a god exists or they weren't sure. Another 12.1% said there is a higher power but no personal god. In total, only 15.0% identified as Nones or No Religion, but 24.4% did not believe in the traditional concept of a personal god. The conductors of the study concluded, "The historic reluctance of Americans to self-identify in this manner or use these terms seems to have diminished. Nevertheless ... the level of under-reporting of these theological labels is still significant ... many millions do not subscribe fully to the theology of the groups with which they identify."Barry A. Kosmin and Ariela Keysar, {{cite web|url=http://b27.cc.trincoll.edu/weblogs/AmericanReligionSurvey-ARIS/reports/ARIS_Report_2008.pdf|title=Archived copy|archiveurl=https://web.archive.org/web/20090407053149/http://b27.cc.trincoll.edu/weblogs/AmericanReligionSurvey-ARIS/reports/ARIS_Report_2008.pdf|archivedate=April 7, 2009|url-status=dead|accessdate=2009-05-08}}, March 2009, American Religious Identification Survey [ARIS 2008], Trinity College.

According to a Pew Research Center study in 2009, only 5% of the total US population did not have a belief in a god. Out of all those without a belief in a god, only 24% self-identified as "atheist", while 15% self-identified as "agnostic", 35% self-identified as "nothing in particular", and 24% identified with a religious tradition.{{cite web|url=http://www.pewforum.org/Not-All-Nonbelievers-Call-Themselves-Atheists.aspx|title=Not All Nonbelievers Call Themselves Atheists | Pew Research Center's Religion & Public Life Project|date=2009-04-02|publisher=Pewforum.org|accessdate=2014-02-27}}

Gallup's editor-in-chief, Frank Newport, argues that numbers on surveys may give an incomplete picture. In his view, declines in religious affiliation or belief in God on surveys may not actually reflect real declines, but instead increased honesty to interviewers on spiritual matters due to viewpoints previously seen as deviant becoming more socially acceptable.{{Cite magazine|last=Johnson|first=David|url=https://time.com/4283975/god-belief-religion-americans/|title=See How Americans' Belief in God Has Changed Over 70 Years|magazine=Time|language=en|access-date=2018-03-24|date=7 April 2016}}

= Censuses =

Questions of religion are "marginal" in censuses, usually optional, and are left out of most censuses in most countries.{{cite journal |last1=Thorvaldsen |first1=Gunnar |title=Religion in the Census |journal=Social Science History |date=2014 |volume=38 |issue=1–2 |pages=203–220 |doi=10.1017/ssh.2015.16}} Despite attempts to standardize wording, census phrasing of the religion question have not been consistent over time or from country to country, with responders understanding them in 3 different ways. Censuses aim to enumerate religious communities, not religious faith, and "as long as the censuses in more than half of the world do not ask about religion it will not be possible to tell even within the closest million the size of the different religious communities globally." Due to the complexity of measuring religious identity, censuses sometimes also overestimate groups; this was the case for Christians in Britain, as typically one person fills out the census one behalf of a household, as distinguished from surveys which ask individual adults.{{cite journal |last1=Voas |first1=David |last2=Bruce |first2=Steve |title=Research note: The 2001 census and christian identification in Britain |journal=Journal of Contemporary Religion |date=January 2004 |volume=19 |issue=1 |pages=23–28 |doi=10.1080/1353790032000165087 |url=https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/1353790032000165087 |language=en |issn=1353-7903|url-access=subscription }}

Causes and correlates

=Genes and environment=

File:Church Attendance and Welfare Spending Graph.png

The contributions of genes and environment to religiosity have been quantified in studies of twins{{cite journal | last1 = Bouchard | first1 = TJ Jr | last2 = McGue | first2 = M | last3 = Lykken | first3 = D | last4 = Tellegen | first4 = A | date = Jun 1999 | title = Intrinsic and extrinsic religiousness: genetic and environmental influences and personality correlates | journal = Twin Res. | volume = 2 | issue = 2| pages = 88–98 | doi = 10.1375/twin.2.2.88 | pmid = 10480743 | doi-access = free }}{{cite journal | last1 = Kirk | first1 = KM | last2 = Eaves | first2 = LJ | last3 = Martin | first3 = NG | date = Jun 1999 | title = Self-transcendence as a measure of spirituality in a sample of older Australian twins | journal = Twin Res. | volume = 2 | issue = 2| pages = 81–7 | pmid = 10480742 | doi = 10.1375/twin.2.2.81 | doi-access = free }} and sociological studies of welfare, availability, and legal regulationsNolan, P., & Lenski, G. E. (2010). Human societies: Introduction to macrosociology. Boulder, CO: Paradigm Publisher. {{isbn|978-1594515781}}. (state religions, etc.).

Koenig and colleagues reported in a 2005 research paper that between adolescence and adulthood, the contribution of genes to variation in religiosity (called heritability) increases from 12% to 44% and the contribution of shared (family) effects decreases from 56% to 18%.{{cite journal | last1 = Koenig | first1 = L. B. | last2 = McGue | first2 = M. | last3 = Krueger | first3 = R. F. | last4 = Bouchard Jr | first4 = T. J. | year = 2005 | title = Genetic and environmental influences on religiousness: findings for retrospective and current religiousness ratings | journal = Journal of Personality | volume = 73| issue = 2 | pages = 471–488| doi = 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00316.x | pmid = 15745438 }}

A market-based theory of religious choice and governmental regulation of religion have been the dominant theories used to explain variations of religiosity between societies{{Clarify|date=January 2017}}. However, researchers Anthony Gill and Eric Lundsgaarde documented a much stronger correlation between welfare state spending and religiosity (see diagram).

=Just-world fallacy=

Studies have found belief in a just world to be correlated with aspects of religiosity.{{cite journal | last1 = Begue | first1 = L | year = 2002 | title = Beliefs in justice and faith in people: just world, religiosity and interpersonal trust | journal = Personality and Individual Differences | volume = 32 | issue = 3| pages = 375–382 | doi = 10.1016/s0191-8869(00)00224-5 }}{{cite journal | last1 = Kurst | first1 = J. | last2 = Bjorck | first2 = J. | last3 = Tan | first3 = S. | year = 2000 | title = Causal attributions for uncontrollable negative events | journal = Journal of Psychology and Christianity | volume = 19 | pages = 47–60 }}

=Risk-aversion=

Several studies have discovered a positive correlation between the degree of religiousness and risk aversion.{{cite journal | title = Risk aversion and religion | journal = Journal of Risk and Uncertainty | first = Charles | last = Noussair |author2=Stefan T. Trautmann|author3=Gijs van de Kuilen|author4=Nathanael Vellekoop | volume = 47| issue = 2 | pages = 165–183 | year = 2013| doi = 10.1007/s11166-013-9174-8| s2cid = 54664945 | url = https://pure.uvt.nl/ws/files/1443326/2012-073.pdf }}.{{cite journal | title = Does local religiosity matter for bank risk-taking? | journal = Journal of Corporate Finance | first = Binay| last = Adhikari|author2=Anup Agrawal| volume = 38|pages = 272–293| year = 2016| doi = 10.1016/j.jcorpfin.2016.01.009}}.

See also

=Demographics=

References

{{Reflist}}