colexification
{{Short description|Multi-meaning encoding in words, comprising vagueness, polysemy, and homonymy}}
In semantics and lexical typology, colexification is the ability for a language to express different meanings with the same word.
When a language colexifies several concepts, this is generally understood as a sign that these concepts are semantically related.The only exception would be cases of homophony, when two semantically unrelated words sound (or look) identical for accidental reasons. Homophony is not central to the study of colexification. Research in lexical typology has thus been using colexification as a tool to measure semantic similarity between concepts.
Definition
Colexification describes the case of different meanings being expressed by the same word (i.e., “co-lexified”) in a language. For example, the two senses which are distinguished in English as people and village are colexified in Spanish, which uses pueblo in both cases.
Colexification is meant as a neutral descriptive term that avoids distinguishing between vagueness, polysemy, and homonymy. Some cases of colexification are common across the world (e.g. ‘blue’ = ‘green’); others are typical of certain linguistic and cultural areas (e.g. ‘tree’ = ‘fire’ among Papuan and Australian languages;See Schapper et al. (2016). or ‘thunder’ = ‘dragon’ in the Sino-Tibetan languagesSee Ding & Dong (2024).).
The opposite of “co-lexify” is “dis-lexify”, i.e. 'express two meanings using different lexical forms'.François (2022:95). Thus, Russian colexifies 'arm' and 'hand' using the single word рука, but Spanish dislexifies these two meanings using two distinct words, respectively brazo v. mano.
Examples
class="wikitable"
|+ !Language !Word form !sense 1 !sense 2 !sense 3 |
Basque
|‘village’ |‘people’ |‘country’ |
Spanish
|‘village’ |‘people’ | |
Catalan
|‘feel’ |‘hear’ | |
rowspan="3" |French
|‘woman’ |‘wife’ | |
fille
|‘girl’ |‘daughter’ | |
grand
|‘large’ |‘tall (in size)’ |‘grown up (in age)’ |
rowspan="2" |English
|‘mother's brother’ |‘father's brother’ |‘aunt's husband’ |
draw
|‘pull, drag’ |‘depict w/ lines’ | |
Kriol
|‘hit’ |‘kill’ | |
Chinese
|‘sky’ |‘heaven’ |‘day’ |
Japanese
|木 ki |‘tree’ |‘wood’ | |
Mota
|pane- |‘arm’ |‘hand’ |‘wing’ |
Italian
|{{lang|vi|{{linktext|ciao|lang=it}}}} |rowspan="2"|‘hello’ |rowspan="2"|‘goodbye’ |
Vietnamese
|{{lang|vi|{{linktext|chào|lang=vi}}}} |
rowspan="2" |LSF
| |[https://projects.lib.rochester.edu/lsf-asl/wp-content/uploads/bonjour.mp4 ‘hello’] |[https://projects.lib.rochester.edu/lsf-asl/wp-content/uploads/merci.mp4 ‘thanks’] | |
[https://signtyp.uconn.edu/signpuddle/glyphogram.php?ksw=M21x85S30004n18xn17S1dc01n12x17S20e009x14S20e00n25x14S26a04n14x49S1f401n12x64 (sign)]
|[https://projects.lib.rochester.edu/lsf-asl/wp-content/uploads/sympathique.mp4 ‘(s.o.) kind, nice’] |[https://projects.lib.rochester.edu/lsf-asl/wp-content/uploads/facile.mp4 ‘(s.th.) easy’] | |
Use in linguistic studies
{{rquote|right|text=“A given language is said to colexify two functionally distinct senses if, and only if, it can associate them with the same lexical form.”|author=François (2008: 170)See also François (2022):95): “A language colexifies two distinct senses if it can express them using the same lexical form.”.}} The term was coined by the linguist Alexandre François in his 2008 article “Semantic maps and the typology of colexification”. This article illustrated the notion with various examples, including the semantic domains of { {{sc|straight}} }, { {{sc|call}} }, { {{sc|breathe}} }. The latter notion is at the source of a colexification network that is attested in several languages, linking together such senses as ‘breath’, ‘life’, ‘soul’, ‘spirit’, ‘ghost’...: Skr. आत्मन् ātmán; Anc. Gk ψυχή, πνεῦμα; Lat. animus, spīritus; Arab. روح rūḥ, etc. François built on that example to propose a method for constructing lexical semantic maps.
Several studies have taken up the concept of colexification and applied it to different semantic domains and various language families.See the bibliography.
Colexification is also the object of a dedicated database, known as [https://clics.clld.org/ CLiCS] “Database of Cross-Linguistic Colexifications”.See List et al. (2018) and Rzymski et al. (2020). Based on data from more than 2400 language varieties of the world, the database makes it possible to check the typological frequency of individual instances of colexification,For example, the [https://clics.clld.org/edges/1079-1408 colexification pair ‘hear’–‘feel’]. and to visualize semantic networksFor example, the [https://clics.clld.org/graphs/subgraph_3 subgraph around the notion { {{sc|brave}} }] based on empirical data from the world's languages.
See also
Notes
{{Reflist}}
References
- {{Cite journal| doi = 10.1016/j.cognition.2022.105179| volume = 226| pages = 105179| last1 = Brochhagen| first1 = Thomas| last2 = Boleda| first2 = Gemma| title = When do languages use the same word for different meanings? The Goldilocks principle in colexification| journal = Cognition| date = 2022| hdl = 10230/56229| hdl-access = free}}
- {{Cite journal| doi = 10.1007/s42761-021-00033-1| last1 = Di Natale| first1 = Anna| last2 = Pellert| first2 = Max| last3 = Garcia| first3 = David| title = Colexification networks encode affective meaning| journal = Affective Science| date = 2021| pmc = 9382918}}
- {{Cite journal| doi = 10.1080/23306343.2024.2383513| pages = 1–23| last1 = Ding| first1 = Hongdi| last2 = Dong| first2 = Sicong| title = Colexification of “thunder” and “dragon” in Sino-Tibetan languages| journal = Asia Pacific Translation and Intercultural Studies |year=2024| ref = DingDong}}
- {{Citation
| last = François
| first = Alexandre
| author-link =
| contribution = Semantic maps and the typology of colexification: Intertwining polysemous networks across languages
| editor-last = Vanhove
| editor-first = Martine
| title = From Polysemy to Semantic change: Towards a Typology of Lexical Semantic Associations
| volume = 106
| pages = 163–215
| publisher = Benjamins
| place = Amsterdam, New York
| year = 2008
| series = Studies in Language Companion Series
| isbn =
| chapter-url= https://www.academia.edu/846905
| ref = AF2008
}}.
- {{cite journal |last1=François |first1=Alexandre |year=2022 |title=Lexical tectonics: Mapping structural change in patterns of lexification |journal=Zeitschrift für Sprachwissenschaft |volume= 41|issue=1 |pages=89‒123 |publisher=DeGruyter |doi=10.1515/zfs-2021-2041 |s2cid=246000538 |url=https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/halshs-03092510 |accessdate=23 June 2022 |ref=tectonics |doi-access=free }}
- Gast, Volker & Maria Koptjevskaja-Tamm. 2018. [https://www.degruyter.com/document/doi/10.1515/9783110607963-003/html The areal factor in lexical typology]. In D. Olmen, T. Mortelmans & F. Brisard (eds), Aspects of linguistic variation, 43–82. Berlin: DeGruyter.
- {{Cite journal| doi = 10.1515/lingty-2021-2088| volume = 26| issue = 2| last1 = Georgakopoulos| first1 = Thanasis| last2 = Grossman| first2 = Eitan| last3 = Nikolaev| first3 = Dmitry| last4 = Polis| first4 = Stéphane| title = Universal and macro-areal patterns in the lexicon: A case-study in the perception-cognition domain| journal = Linguistic Typology| date = 2021| doi-access = free}}
- Georgakopoulos, Thanasis, Daniel A. Werning, Jörg Hartlieb, Tomoki Kitazumi, Lidewij van de Peut, Annette Sundermeyer & Gaëlle Chantrain. 2016. [http://journal.topoi.org/index.php/etopoi/article/view/261 The meaning of ancient words for ‘earth’: An exercise in visualizing colexification on a semantic map] {{Webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20200713101039/http://journal.topoi.org/index.php/etopoi/article/view/261 |date=2020-07-13 }}. In Gerd Graßhoff & Michael Meyer (eds), Space and Knowledge. Special issue of eTopoi. Journal for Ancient Studies 6. 418–452.
- {{cite journal
| first1 = J. | last1 = Jackson
| first2 = J. | last2 = Watts
| first3 = T. | last3 = Henry
| first4 = J.-M. | last4 = List
| first5 = P. | last5 = Mucha
| first6 = R. | last6 = Forkel | first7 = S. | last7 = Greenhill | first8 = K. | last8 = Lindquist
| year=2019
| title= Emotion semantics show both cultural variation and universal structure
| journal = Science
| volume = 366
| issue = 6472
| pages = 1517–1522
| doi = 10.1126/science.aaw8160
| pmid = 31857485
| bibcode = 2019Sci...366.1517J
| s2cid = 209424412
| url=https://www.science.org/doi/10.1126/science.aaw8160
| hdl = 1885/220045| hdl-access = free}}
- {{Cite book| publisher = De Gruyter |isbn=9783110377675| pages = | editor1-first = Päivi | editor1-last = Juvonen| editor2 =Maria Koptjevskaja-Tamm | title = The Lexical Typology of Semantic Shifts| location = Berlin, Boston| date = 2016|ref=}}
- {{Cite journal| doi = 10.1111/cogs.13035| volume = 45| issue = 9| last1 = Karjus| first1 = Andres| last2 = Blythe| first2 = Richard A.| last3 = Kirby| first3 = Simon| last4 = Wang| first4 = Tianyu| last5 = Smith| first5 = Kenny| title = Conceptual similarity and communicative need shape colexification: An experimental study| journal = Cognitive Science| date = 2021| arxiv = 2103.11024}}
- {{ cite journal
| last1 = List | first1 = Johann-Mattis
| first2 = Simon | last2 = Greenhill
| first3 = Cormac | last3 = Anderson
| first4 = Thomas | last4 = Mayer
| first5 = Tiago | last5 = Tresoldi
| first6 = Robert | last6 = Forkel
| year = 2018
| title = CLiCS²: An improved database of cross-linguistic colexifications assembling lexical data with the help of cross-linguistic data formats
| journal = Linguistic Typology
| doi = 10.22425/jul.2015.16.2.63
| pages = 277–306
| volume = 22
| number = 2
| doi-access = free
}}
- Pericliev, Vladimir. 2015. On colexification among basic vocabulary. Journal of Universal Language 16(2). 63–93. {{doi|10.22425/jul.2015.16.2.63}}.
- {{ cite journal
| first1 = C. | last1 = Rzymski
| first2 = T. | last2 = Tresoldi
| first3 = S. | last3 = Greenhill
| first4 = M. | last4 = Wu
| first5 = N. | last5 = Schweikhard
| first6 = M. | last6 = Koptjevskaja-Tamm
| first7 = V. | last7 = Gast
| first8 = T. | last8 = Bodt
| first9 = A. | last9 = Hantgan
| first10 = G. | last10 = Kaiping
| first11 = S. | last11 = Chang
| first12 = Y. | last12 = Lai
| first13 = N. | last13 = Morozova
| first14 = H. | last14 = Arjava
| first15 = N. | last15 = Hübler
| first16 = E. | last16 = Koile
| first17 = S. | last17 = Pepper
| first18 = M. | last18 = Proos
| first19 = B. | last19 = Epps
| first20 = I. | last20 = Blanco
| first21 = C. | last21 = Hundt
| first22 = S. | last22 = Monakhov
| first23 = K. | last23 = Pianykh
| first24 = S. | last24 = Ramesh
| first25 = R. | last25 = Gray
| first26 = R. | last26 = Forkel
| first27 = J.-M. | last27 = List
| doi = 10.1038/s41597-019-0341-x
| pmid = 31932593
| journal = Scientific Data
| title = The Database of Cross-Linguistic Colexifications, reproducible analysis of cross- linguistic polysemies
| volume = 7
| number = 13
| year = 2020
| page = 13
| pmc = 6957499
| bibcode = 2020NatSD...7...13R
| doi-access = free
}}
- {{Cite book| publisher = De Gruyter |doi=10.1515/9783110377675-012| pages = 355–422| editor1 = Päivi Juvonen| editor2 =Maria Koptjevskaja-Tamm| last1 = Schapper| first1 = Antoinette| last2 = San Roque| first2 = Lila| last3 = Hendery| first3 = Rachel| title = The Lexical Typology of Semantic Shifts| chapter = Tree, firewood and fire in the languages of Sahul| location = Berlin, Boston| date = 2016| chapter-url = https://www.degruyter.com/view/books/9783110377675/9783110377675-012/9783110377675-012.xml |ref=schapper}}
- Urban, Matthias. 2012. Analyzability and semantic associations in referring expressions. Leiden University PhD dissertation.
External links
- [https://clics.clld.org/ CLiCS] “Database of Cross-Linguistic Colexifications”.