Broca's area
{{short description|Speech production region in the dominant hemisphere of the hominid brain}}
{{Infobox brain
| Name = Broca's area
| Latin =
| Image = Broca’s area - BA44 and BA45.png
| Caption = Broca's area is made up of Brodmann areas 44 (pars opercularis) and 45 (pars triangularis).
| Image2 = Broca's area - lateral view.png
| Caption2 = Broca's area (shown in red)
| IsPartOf = Frontal lobe
| Components =
| Artery = Middle cerebral
| Vein = Superior sagittal sinus
}}
Broca's area, or the Broca area ({{IPAc-en|ˈ|b|r|oʊ|k|ə}},{{Cite American Heritage Dictionary|Broca's area|access-date=8 September 2019}}{{Cite dictionary |url=http://www.lexico.com/definition/Broca%27s+area |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20220826080617/https://www.dictionary.com/ |url-status=dead |archive-date=2022-08-26 |title=Broca's area |dictionary=Lexico UK English Dictionary |publisher=Oxford University Press}}{{Cite Merriam-Webster|Broca's area|access-date=8 September 2019}} also {{IPAc-en|UK|ˈ|b|r|ɒ|k|ə}}, {{IPAc-en|US|ˈ|b|r|oʊ|k|ɑː}}{{cite web|url=https://www.collinsdictionary.com/dictionary/english/brocas-area|title=Broca's area|work=Collins English Dictionary|publisher=HarperCollins|access-date=8 September 2019}}), is a region in the frontal lobe of the dominant hemisphere, usually the left, of the brain{{cite journal | vauthors = Cantalupo C, Hopkins WD | title = Asymmetric Broca's area in great apes | journal = Nature | volume = 414 | issue = 6863 | pages = 505 | date = November 2001 | pmid = 11734839 | pmc = 2043144 | doi = 10.1038/35107134 | bibcode = 2001Natur.414..505C }} with functions linked to speech production.
Language processing has been linked to Broca's area since Pierre Paul Broca reported impairments in two patients.{{cite book |last=Kennison |first=Sheila |year=2013 |title=Introduction to language development |location=Los Angeles |publisher=Sage|isbn=978-1-4522-5629-0}}{{page needed|date=January 2015}} They had lost the ability to speak after injury to the posterior inferior frontal gyrus (pars triangularis) (BA45) of the brain.{{cite journal | vauthors = Dronkers NF, Plaisant O, Iba-Zizen MT, Cabanis EA | title = Paul Broca's historic cases: high resolution MR imaging of the brains of Leborgne and Lelong | journal = Brain | volume = 130 | issue = Pt 5 | pages = 1432–41 | date = May 2007 | pmid = 17405763 | doi = 10.1093/brain/awm042 | doi-access = free }} Since then, the approximate region he identified has become known as Broca's area, and the deficit in language production as Broca's aphasia, also called expressive aphasia. Broca's area is now typically defined in terms of the pars opercularis and pars triangularis of the inferior frontal gyrus, represented in Brodmann's cytoarchitectonic map as Brodmann area 44 and Brodmann area 45 of the dominant hemisphere.
Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) has shown language processing to also involve the third part of the inferior frontal gyrus the pars orbitalis, as well as the ventral part of BA6 and these are now often included in a larger area called Broca's region.{{cite journal |author=Hagoort, P. |title=Nodes and networks in the neural architecture for language: Broca's region and beyond |journal=Current Opinion in Neurobiology |volume=28 |year=2014 |pages=136–141 |url=http://pubman.mpdl.mpg.de/pubman/item/escidoc:2044287/component/escidoc:2044290/hagoort_2014.pdf |archive-url=https://ghostarchive.org/archive/20221009/http://pubman.mpdl.mpg.de/pubman/item/escidoc:2044287/component/escidoc:2044290/hagoort_2014.pdf |archive-date=2022-10-09 |url-status=live |doi=10.1016/j.conb.2014.07.013 |pmid=25062474|hdl=11858/00-001M-0000-0019-FEFE-C |s2cid=16858289 |hdl-access=free }}
Studies of chronic aphasia have implicated an essential role of Broca's area in various speech and language functions. Further, fMRI studies have also identified activation patterns in Broca's area associated with various language tasks. However, slow destruction of Broca's area by brain tumors can leave speech relatively intact, suggesting its functions can shift to nearby areas in the brain.{{cite journal | vauthors = Plaza M, Gatignol P, Leroy M, Duffau H | title = Speaking without Broca's area after tumor resection | journal = Neurocase | volume = 15 | issue = 4 | pages = 294–310 | date = August 2009 | pmid = 19274574 | doi = 10.1080/13554790902729473 | s2cid = 16683208 }}
Structure
File:Brodmann Cytoarchitectonics 44.png]]
File:Brodmann Cytoarchitectonics 45.png]]
Broca's area is often identified by visual inspection of the topography of the brain either by macrostructural landmarks such as sulci or by the specification of coordinates in a particular reference space. The currently used Talairach and Tournoux atlas projects Brodmann's cytoarchitectonic map onto a template brain. Because Brodmann's parcelation was based on subjective visual inspection of cytoarchitectonic borders and also Brodmann analyzed only one hemisphere of one brain, the result is imprecise. Further, because of considerable variability across brains in terms of shape, size, and position relative to sulcal and gyral structure, a resulting localization precision is limited.{{cite journal | vauthors = Grodzinsky Y, Santi A | title = The battle for Broca's region | journal = Trends in Cognitive Sciences | volume = 12 | issue = 12 | pages = 474–80 | date = December 2008 | pmid = 18930695 | doi = 10.1016/j.tics.2008.09.001 | s2cid = 13023258 }}
Nevertheless, Broca's area in the left hemisphere and its homologue in the right hemisphere are designations usually used to refer to the triangular part of inferior frontal gyrus (PTr) and the opercular part of inferior frontal gyrus (POp). The PTr and POp are defined by structural landmarks that only probabilistically divide the inferior frontal gyrus into anterior and posterior cytoarchitectonic areas of 45 and 44, respectively, by Brodmann's classification scheme.{{cite journal | vauthors = Skipper JI, Goldin-Meadow S, Nusbaum HC, Small SL | title = Speech-associated gestures, Broca's area, and the human mirror system | journal = Brain and Language | volume = 101 | issue = 3 | pages = 260–77 | date = June 2007 | pmid = 17533001 | pmc = 2703472 | doi = 10.1016/j.bandl.2007.02.008 }}
Area 45 receives more afferent connections from the prefrontal cortex, the superior temporal gyrus, and the superior temporal sulcus, compared to area 44, which tends to receive more afferent connections from motor, somatosensory, and inferior parietal regions.
The differences between area 45 and 44 in cytoarchitecture and in connectivity suggest that these areas might perform different functions. Indeed, recent neuroimaging studies have shown that the PTr and Pop, corresponding to areas 45 and 44, respectively, play different functional roles in the human with respect to language comprehension and action recognition/understanding.
The Broca's area is about 20% larger in women than in men.{{cite journal | vauthors = Schlaepfer TE, Harris GJ, Tien AY, Peng L, Lee S, Pearlson GD | year = 1995 | title = Structural differences in the cerebral cortex of healthy female and male subjects: a magnetic resonance imaging study | journal = Psychiatry Res | volume = 61 | issue = 3| pages = 129–35 | doi = 10.1016/0925-4927(95)02634-a | pmid = 8545497 | s2cid = 25425465 | doi-access = free }}
Functions
=Language comprehension=
For a long time, it was assumed that the role of Broca's area was more devoted to language production than language comprehension. However, there is evidence to demonstrate that Broca's area also plays a significant role in language comprehension. Patients with lesions in Broca's area who exhibit agrammatical speech production also show inability to use syntactic information to determine the meaning of sentences.{{cite journal | vauthors = Caplan D | title = Why is Broca's area involved in syntax? | journal = Cortex; A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior | volume = 42 | issue = 4 | pages = 469–71 | date = May 2006 | pmid = 16881251 | doi = 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70379-4 | s2cid = 4480694 }} Also, a number of neuroimaging studies have implicated an involvement of Broca's area, particularly of the pars opercularis of the left inferior frontal gyrus, during the processing of complex sentences.{{cite journal | vauthors = Grewe T, Bornkessel I, Zysset S, Wiese R, von Cramon DY, Schlesewsky M | title = The emergence of the unmarked: a new perspective on the language-specific function of Broca's area | journal = Human Brain Mapping | volume = 26 | issue = 3 | pages = 178–90 | date = November 2005 | pmid = 15929098 | doi = 10.1002/hbm.20154 | url = http://edoc.mpg.de/244878 | pmc = 6871720 }} Further, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) experiments have shown that highly ambiguous sentences result in a more activated inferior frontal gyrus.{{cite journal | vauthors = Rodd JM, Davis MH, Johnsrude IS | title = The neural mechanisms of speech comprehension: fMRI studies of semantic ambiguity | journal = Cerebral Cortex | volume = 15 | issue = 8 | pages = 1261–9 | date = August 2005 | pmid = 15635062 | doi = 10.1093/cercor/bhi009 | doi-access = free }} Therefore, the activity level in the inferior frontal gyrus and the level of lexical ambiguity are directly proportional to each other, because of the increased retrieval demands associated with highly ambiguous content.
There is also specialisation for particular aspects of comprehension within Broca's area. Work by Devlin et al. (2003){{cite journal | vauthors = Devlin JT, Matthews PM, Rushworth MF | title = Semantic processing in the left inferior prefrontal cortex: a combined functional magnetic resonance imaging and transcranial magnetic stimulation study | journal = Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience | volume = 15 | issue = 1 | pages = 71–84 | date = January 2003 | pmid = 12590844 | doi = 10.1162/089892903321107837 | citeseerx = 10.1.1.329.8485 | s2cid = 7570128 }} showed in a repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) study that there was an increase in reaction times when performing a semantic task under rTMS aimed at the pars triangularis (situated in the anterior part of Broca's area). The increase in reaction times is indicative that that particular area is responsible for processing that cognitive function. Disrupting these areas via TMS disrupts computations performed in the areas leading to an increase in time needed to perform the computations (reflected in reaction times). Later work by Nixon et al. (2004){{cite journal | vauthors = Nixon P, Lazarova J, Hodinott-Hill I, Gough P, Passingham R | title = The inferior frontal gyrus and phonological processing: an investigation using rTMS | journal = Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience | volume = 16 | issue = 2 | pages = 289–300 | date = March 2004 | pmid = 15068598 | doi = 10.1162/089892904322984571 | s2cid = 1162060 | url = http://eprints.maynoothuniversity.ie/4866/1/PG_rTMS.pdf }} showed that when the pars opercularis (situated in the posterior part of Broca's area) was stimulated under rTMS there was an increase in reaction times in a phonological task. Gough et al. (2005){{cite journal | vauthors = Gough PM, Nobre AC, Devlin JT | title = Dissociating linguistic processes in the left inferior frontal cortex with transcranial magnetic stimulation | journal = The Journal of Neuroscience | volume = 25 | issue = 35 | pages = 8010–6 | date = August 2005 | pmid = 16135758 | pmc = 1403818 | doi = 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2307-05.2005 }} performed an experiment combining elements of these previous works in which both phonological and semantic tasks were performed with rTMS stimulation directed at either the anterior or the posterior part of Broca's area. The results from this experiment conclusively distinguished anatomical specialisation within Broca's area for different components of language comprehension. Here the results showed that under rTMS stimulation:
- Semantic tasks only showed a decrease in reaction times when stimulation was aimed at the anterior part of Broca's area (where a decrease of 10% (50 ms) was seen compared to a no-TMS control group)
- Phonological tasks showed a decrease in reaction times when stimulation was aimed at the posterior part of Broca's area (where a decrease of 6% (30 ms) was seen compared to control)
To summarise, the work above shows anatomical specialisation in Broca's area for language comprehension, with the anterior part of Broca's area responsible for understanding the meaning of words (semantics) and the posterior part of Broca's area responsible for understanding how words sound (phonology).
=Action recognition and production=
Experiments have indicated that Broca's area is involved in various cognitive and perceptual tasks. One important contribution of Brodmann's area 44 is also found in the motor-related processes. Observation of meaningful hand shadows resembling moving animals activates frontal language area, demonstrating that Broca's area indeed plays a role in interpreting action of others.{{cite journal | vauthors = Fadiga L, Craighero L, Destro MF, Finos L, Cotillon-Williams N, Smith AT, Castiello U | title = Language in shadow | journal = Social Neuroscience | volume = 1 | issue = 2 | pages = 77–89 | year = 2006 | pmid = 18633777 | doi = 10.1080/17470910600976430 | s2cid = 20322 }} An activation of BA 44 was also reported during execution of grasping and manipulation.{{cite journal | vauthors = Fadiga L, Craighero L | title = Hand actions and speech representation in Broca's area | journal = Cortex; A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior | volume = 42 | issue = 4 | pages = 486–90 | date = May 2006 | pmid = 16881255 | doi = 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70383-6 | s2cid = 2463077 }}
=Speech-associated gestures=
It has been speculated that because speech-associated gestures could possibly reduce lexical or sentential ambiguity, comprehension should improve in the presence of speech-associated gestures. As a result of improved comprehension, the involvement of Broca's area should be reduced.
Many neuroimaging studies have also shown activation of Broca's area when representing meaningful arm gestures. A recent study has shown evidence that word and gesture are related at the level of translation of particular gesture aspects such as its motor goal and intention.{{cite journal | vauthors = Gentilucci M, Bernardis P, Crisi G, Dalla Volta R | title = Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of Broca's area affects verbal responses to gesture observation | journal = Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience | volume = 18 | issue = 7 | pages = 1059–74 | date = July 2006 | pmid = 16839281 | doi = 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.7.1059 | s2cid = 18159912 }} This finding helps explain why, when this area is defective, those who use sign language also have language deficits.{{cite book |last1=Carlson |first1=N. |year=2013 |chapter=Human Communication |title=Physiology of Behavior |edition=11th |location=Boston |publisher=Allyn and Bacon }}{{rp|494–7}} This finding, that aspects of gestures are translated in words within Broca's area, also explains language development in terms of evolution. Indeed, many authors have proposed that speech evolved from a primitive communication that arose from gestures.{{cite journal | vauthors = Lieberman P | title = On the nature and evolution of the neural bases of human language | journal = American Journal of Physical Anthropology | volume = Suppl 35 | pages = 36–62 | year = 2002 | pmid = 12653308 | doi = 10.1002/ajpa.10171 | doi-access = free }} (See below.)
=Speaking without Broca's area=
Damage to Broca's area is commonly associated with telegraphic speech made up of content vocabulary. For example, a person with Broca's aphasia may say something like, "Drive, store. Mom." meaning to say, "My mom drove me to the store today." Therefore, the content of the information is correct, but the grammar and fluidity of the sentence is missing.{{Cite web | url=http://www.asha.org/PRPSpecificTopic.aspx?folderid=8589934663§ion=Signs_and_Symptoms |title = Aphasia: Signs & Symptoms}}
The essential role of the Broca's area in speech production has been questioned since it can be destroyed while leaving language nearly intact. In one case of a computer engineer, a slow-growing glioma tumor was removed. The tumor and the surgery destroyed the left inferior and middle frontal gyrus, the head of the caudate nucleus, the anterior limb of the internal capsule, and the anterior insula. However, there were minimal language problems three months after removal and the individual returned to his professional work. These minor problems include the inability to create syntactically complex sentences including more than two subjects, multiple causal conjunctions, or reported speech. These were explained by researchers as due to working memory problems. They also attributed his lack of problems to extensive compensatory mechanisms enabled by neural plasticity in the nearby cerebral cortex and a shift of some functions to the homologous area in the right hemisphere.
Clinical significance
=Stuttering=
A speech disorder known as stuttering is seen to be associated with underactivity in Broca's area.Maguire et al. 1994{{full citation needed|date=March 2018}}{{cite journal | vauthors = Maguire EA, Frackowiak RS, Frith CD | title = Recalling routes around london: activation of the right hippocampus in taxi drivers | journal = The Journal of Neuroscience | volume = 17 | issue = 18 | pages = 7103–10 | date = September 1997 | pmid = 9278544 | pmc = 6573257 | doi = 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-18-07103.1997 }}
=Aphasia=
Aphasia is an acquired language disorder affecting all modalities such as writing, reading, speaking, and listening and results from brain damage. It is often a chronic condition that creates changes in all areas of one's life.{{Cite web|url=http://www.atlantaaphasia.org|title=What is Aphasia|access-date=2008-12-01|publisher=Atlanta Aphasia Association|year=2006|url-status=live|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20081223230955/http://www.atlantaaphasia.org/|archive-date=2008-12-23}}
=Expressive aphasia vs. other aphasias=
Patients with expressive aphasia, also known as Broca's aphasia, are individuals who know "what they want to say, they just cannot get it out". They are typically able to comprehend words, and sentences with a simple syntactic structure (see above), but are more or less unable to generate fluent speech. Other symptoms that may be present include problems with fluency, articulation, word-finding, word repetition, and producing and comprehending complex grammatical sentences, both orally and in writing.
This specific group of symptoms distinguishes those who have expressive aphasia from individuals with other types of aphasia. There are several distinct "types" of aphasia, and each type is characterized by a different set of language deficits. Although those who have expressive aphasia tend to retain good spoken language comprehension, other types of aphasia can render patients completely unable to understand any language at all, unable to understand any spoken language (auditory verbal agnosia),{{cite journal | vauthors = Metz-Lutz MN, Dahl E | title = Analysis of word comprehension in a case of pure word deafness | journal = Brain and Language | volume = 23 | issue = 1 | pages = 13–25 | date = September 1984 | pmid = 6478188 | doi = 10.1016/0093-934X(84)90002-6 | s2cid = 39218546 }}{{cite journal | vauthors = Slevc LR, Martin RC, Hamilton AC, Joanisse MF | title = Speech perception, rapid temporal processing, and the left hemisphere: a case study of unilateral pure word deafness | journal = Neuropsychologia | volume = 49 | issue = 2 | pages = 216–30 | date = January 2011 | pmid = 21093464 | pmc = 3031136 | doi = 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.11.009 }}{{cite journal |doi=10.1207/s15516709cog2505_3 |title=Pure word deafness and the bilateral processing of the speech code |journal=Cognitive Science |volume=25 |issue=5 |pages=679–93 |year=2001 |last1=Poeppel |first1=David |doi-access=free }} whereas still other types preserve language comprehension, but with deficits. People with expressive aphasia may struggle less with reading and writing (see alexia) than those with other types of aphasia.{{rp|480–500}} Although individuals with expressive aphasia tend to have a good ability to self-monitor their language output (they "hear what they say" and make corrections), other types of aphasics can seem entirely unaware of their language deficits.
In the classical sense, expressive aphasia is the result of injury to Broca's area; it is often the case that lesions in specific brain areas cause specific, dissociable symptoms,{{cite web|url=http://www.aphasia.org/Aphasia%20Facts/aphasia_facts.html|title=The National Aphasia Foundation|access-date=January 15, 2011|url-status=dead|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20110122211354/http://aphasia.org/Aphasia%20Facts/aphasia_facts.html|archive-date=January 22, 2011}} although case studies show there is not always a one-to-one mapping between lesion location and aphasic symptoms. The correlation between damage to certain specific brain areas (usually in the left hemisphere) and the development of specific types of aphasia makes it possible to deduce (albeit very roughly) the location of a suspected brain lesion based only on the presence (and severity) of a certain type of aphasia, though this is complicated by the possibility that a patient may have damage to a number of brain areas and may exhibit symptoms of more than one type of aphasia. The examination of lesion data in order to deduce which brain areas are essential in the normal functioning of certain aspects of cognition is called the deficit-lesion method; this method is especially important in the branch of neuroscience known as aphasiology. Cognitive science – to be specific, cognitive neuropsychology – are branches of neuroscience that also make extensive use of the deficit-lesion method.{{cite book |chapter=Evaluating Techniques for the Study of Brain Damage |chapter-url={{Google books|wGti6_4Qn_QC|page=165|plainurl=yes}} |pages=[https://archive.org/details/cognitivescience0000frie/page/165 165–6] |title=Cognitive science: an introduction to the study of mind |isbn=978-1-4129-2568-6 |last1=Friedenberg |first1=Jay |last2=Silverman |first2=Gordon |name-list-style=vanc |year=2006 |url=https://archive.org/details/cognitivescience0000frie/page/165 }}
class="wikitable sortable"
|+ Major characteristics of different types of acute aphasia ! Type of aphasia ! Naming ! Auditory comprehension ! Fluency |
Expressive aphasia
| Moderate–severe | Moderate–severe | Mild difficulty | Non-fluent, effortful, slow |
Receptive aphasia
| Mild–severe | Mild–severe | Defective | Fluent paraphasic |
Conduction aphasia
| Poor | Poor | Relatively good | Fluent |
Mixed transcortical aphasia
| Moderate | Poor | Poor | Non-fluent |
Transcortical motor aphasia
| Good | Mild–severe | Mild | Non-fluent |
Transcortical sensory aphasia
| Good | Moderate–severe | Poor | Fluent |
Global aphasia
| Poor | Poor | Poor | Non-fluent |
Anomic aphasia
| Mild | Moderate–severe | Mild | Fluent |
==Broca's area as a key center in the linking of phonemic sequences==
Broca's area has been previously associated with a variety of processes, including phonological segmentation, syntactic processing, and unification, all of which involve segmenting and linking different types of linguistic information.{{cite journal | vauthors = Friederici AD | title = Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence processing | journal = Trends in Cognitive Sciences | volume = 6 | issue = 2 | pages = 78–84 | date = February 2002 | pmid = 15866191 | doi = 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01839-8 | doi-access = free | hdl = 11858/00-001M-0000-0010-E573-8 | hdl-access = free }}{{cite journal | vauthors = Burton MW, Small SL, Blumstein SE | title = The role of segmentation in phonological processing: an fMRI investigation | journal = Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience | volume = 12 | issue = 4 | pages = 679–90 | date = July 2000 | pmid = 10936919 | doi = 10.1162/089892900562309 | s2cid = 685383 }}{{cite journal | vauthors = Flinker A, Chang EF, Barbaro NM, Berger MS, Knight RT | title = Sub-centimeter language organization in the human temporal lobe | journal = Brain and Language | volume = 117 | issue = 3 | pages = 103–9 | date = June 2011 | pmid = 20961611 | pmc = 3025271 | doi = 10.1016/j.bandl.2010.09.009 }} Although repeating and reading single words does not engage semantic and syntactic processing, it does require an operation linking phonemic sequences with motor gestures. Findings indicate that this linkage is coordinated by Broca's area through reciprocal interactions with temporal and frontal cortices responsible for phonemic and articulatory representations, respectively, including interactions with the motor cortex before the actual act of speech. Based on these unique findings, it has been proposed{{by whom|date=March 2018}} that Broca's area is not the seat of articulation, but rather is a key node in manipulating and forwarding neural information across large-scale cortical networks responsible for key components of speech production.{{citation needed|date=March 2018}}
History
In a study published in 2007, the preserved brains of both Leborgne and Lelong (patients of Broca) were reinspected using high-resolution volumetric MRI. The purpose of this study was to scan the brains in three dimensions and to identify the extent of both cortical and subcortical lesions in more detail. The study also sought to locate the exact site of the lesion in the frontal lobe in relation to what is now called Broca's area with the extent of subcortical involvement.
=Broca's patients=
==Louis Victor Leborgne (Tan)==
Leborgne was a patient of Broca's. At 30 years old, he was almost completely unable to produce any words or phrases.{{Cite news|url=https://blogs.scientificamerican.com/literally-psyched/the-man-who-couldnt-speakand-how-he-revolutionized-psychology/|title=The man who couldn t speak and how he revolutionized psychology|last=Konnikova|first=Maria|work=Scientific American Blog Network|access-date=2017-05-03|language=en|url-status=live|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20160914204723/http://blogs.scientificamerican.com/literally-psyched/the-man-who-couldnt-speakand-how-he-revolutionized-psychology/|archive-date=2016-09-14}} He was able to repetitively produce only the word {{Lang|fr|temps}} (French word for "time"). After his death, a neurosyphilitic lesion was discovered on the surface of his left frontal lobe.
==Lelong==
Lelong was another patient of Broca's. He also exhibited reduced productive speech. He could only say five words, 'yes', 'no', 'three', 'always', and 'lelo' (a mispronunciation of his own name). A lesion within the lateral frontal lobe was discovered during Lelong's autopsy. Broca's previous patient, Leborgne, had a lesion in the same area of his frontal lobe. These two cases led Broca to believe that speech was localized to this particular area.
=MRI findings=
Examination of the brains of Broca's two historic patients with high-resolution MRI has produced several interesting findings. First, the MRI findings suggest that other areas besides Broca's area may also have contributed to the patients' reduced productive speech. This finding is significant because it has been found that, though lesions to Broca's area alone can possibly cause temporary speech disruption, they do not result in severe speech arrest. Therefore, there is a possibility that the aphasia denoted by Broca as an absence of productive speech also could have been influenced by the lesions in the other region.{{Citation needed|date=August 2015}} Another finding is that the region, which was once considered to be critical for speech by Broca, is not precisely the same region as what is now known as Broca's area. This study provides further evidence to support the claim that language and cognition are far more complicated than once thought and involve various networks of brain regions.{{Cite web|url=http://memory.ucsf.edu/brain/language/anatomy|title=Anatomy of Speech & Language {{!}} UCSF Memory and Aging Center|website=memory.ucsf.edu|language=en|access-date=2017-05-03|url-status=live|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20170503173358/http://memory.ucsf.edu/brain/language/anatomy|archive-date=2017-05-03}}
Evolution of language
The pursuit of a satisfying theory that addresses the origin of language in humans has led to the consideration of a number of evolutionary "models". These models attempt to show how modern language might have evolved, and a common feature of many of these theories is the idea that vocal communication was initially used to complement a far more dominant mode of communication through gesture. Human language might have evolved as the "evolutionary refinement of an implicit communication system already present in lower primates, based on a set of hand/mouth goal-directed action representations."
"Hand/mouth goal-directed action representations" is another way of saying "gestural communication", "gestural language", or "communication through body language". The recent finding that Broca's area is active when people are observing others engaged in meaningful action is evidence in support of this idea. It was hypothesized that a precursor to the modern Broca's area was involved in translating gestures into abstract ideas by interpreting the movements of others as meaningful action with an intelligent purpose. It is argued that over time the ability to predict the intended outcome and purpose of a set of movements eventually gave this area the capability to deal with truly abstract ideas, and therefore (eventually) became capable of associating sounds (words) with abstract meanings. The observation that frontal language areas are activated when people observe hand shadows is further evidence that human language may have evolved from existing neural substrates that evolved for the purpose of gesture recognition.{{cite journal | vauthors = Corballis MC | title = From mouth to hand: gesture, speech, and the evolution of right-handedness | journal = The Behavioral and Brain Sciences | volume = 26 | issue = 2 | pages = 199–208; discussion 208–60 | date = April 2003 | pmid = 14621511 | doi = 10.1017/S0140525X03000062 | s2cid = 21861033 }} The study, therefore, claims that Broca's area is the "motor center for speech", which assembles and decodes speech sounds in the same way it interprets body language and gestures. Consistent with this idea is that the neural substrate that regulated motor control in the common ancestor of apes and humans was most likely modified to enhance cognitive and linguistic ability. Studies of speakers of American Sign Language and English suggest that the human brain recruited systems that had evolved to perform more basic functions much earlier; these various brain circuits, according to the authors, were tapped to work together in creating language.{{cite journal | vauthors = Newman AJ, Supalla T, Hauser P, Newport EL, Bavelier D | title = Dissociating neural subsystems for grammar by contrasting word order and inflection | journal = Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America | volume = 107 | issue = 16 | pages = 7539–44 | date = April 2010 | pmid = 20368422 | pmc = 2867749 | doi = 10.1073/pnas.1003174107 | bibcode = 2010PNAS..107.7539N | doi-access = free }}
- {{cite press release |date=April 30, 2010 |title=Sign language study shows multiple brain regions wired for language |website=ScienceDaily |url=https://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2010/04/100429173005.htm}}
Another recent finding has shown significant areas of activation in subcortical and neocortical areas during the production of communicative manual gestures and vocal signals in chimpanzees.{{cite journal | vauthors = Taglialatela JP, Russell JL, Schaeffer JA, Hopkins WD | title = Communicative signaling activates 'Broca's' homolog in chimpanzees | journal = Current Biology | volume = 18 | issue = 5 | pages = 343–8 | date = March 2008 | pmid = 18308569 | pmc = 2665181 | doi = 10.1016/j.cub.2008.01.049 | bibcode = 2008CBio...18..343T }} Further, the data indicating that chimpanzees intentionally produce manual gestures as well as vocal signals to communicate with humans suggests that the precursors to human language are present at both the behavioral and neuronanatomical levels. More recently, the neocortical distribution of activity-dependent gene expression in marmosets provided direct evidence that the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, which comprises Broca's area in humans and has been associated with auditory processing of species-specific vocalizations and orofacial control in macaques, is engaged during vocal output in a New World monkey.{{cite journal | vauthors = Simões CS, Vianney PV, de Moura MM, Freire MA, Mello LE, Sameshima K, Araújo JF, Nicolelis MA, Mello CV, Ribeiro S | title = Activation of frontal neocortical areas by vocal production in marmosets | journal = Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience | volume = 4 | year = 2010 | pmid = 20953246 | pmc = 2955454 | doi = 10.3389/fnint.2010.00123 | doi-access = free }}{{cite journal | vauthors = Miller CT, Dimauro A, Pistorio A, Hendry S, Wang X | title = Vocalization Induced CFos Expression in Marmoset Cortex | journal = Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience | volume = 4 | pages = 128 | year = 2010 | pmid = 21179582 | pmc = 3004388 | doi = 10.3389/fnint.2010.00128 | doi-access = free }} These findings putatively set the origin of vocalization-related neocortical circuits to at least 35 million years ago, when the Old and New World monkey lineages split.
Additional images
File:Broca's area animation.gif|Broca's area (shown in red). Animation.
File:Brain - Broca's and Wernicke's area Diagram.svg|Approximate location of Broca's area highlighted in white.
File:The classical Wernicke-Lichtheim-Geschwind model of the neurobiology of language fpsyg-04-00416-g001.jpg|Arcuate fasciculus connects Broca's area and Wernicke's area.
File:Cerebral Hemisphere Demonstration - Sanjoy Sanyal - Neuroscience Lab Fall 2013 (Cropped from 7m45s to 8m9s) - Broca's area.webm|Human brain dissection video (24 sec). Demonstrating the location of Broca's area in inferior frontal gyrus.
File:Broca's area sagittal sections.gif|Sagittal sections of Broca's area
File:Brocas area coronal sections.gif|Coronal sections of Broca's area
File:Broca's area transversal sections.gif|Transversal sections of Broca's area
See also
References
{{Reflist}}
External links
{{Commons category|Broca's area}}
- "Paul Broca's discovery of the area of the brain governing articulated language", analysis of Broca's 1861 article, on [http://www.bibnum.education.fr/sciencesdelavie/neurologie/l-aphasie-de-broca BibNum] [click 'à télécharger' for English version].
{{Telencephalon}}
{{DEFAULTSORT:Broca'S Area}}