Color vision#Physiology of color perception#Theories

{{Short description|Ability to perceive differences in light frequency}}

{{Use American English|date=August 2021}}

File:Filterstef.JPG, and red photographic filters as imaged by camera]]

Color vision, a feature of visual perception, is an ability to perceive differences between light composed of different frequencies independently of light intensity.

Color perception is a part of the larger visual system and is mediated by a complex process between neurons that begins with differential stimulation of different types of photoreceptors by light entering the eye. Those photoreceptors then emit outputs that are propagated through many layers of neurons ultimately leading to higher cognitive functions in the brain. Color vision is found in many animals and is mediated by similar underlying mechanisms with common types of biological molecules and a complex history of the evolution of color vision within different animal taxa. In primates, color vision may have evolved under selective pressure for a variety of visual tasks including the foraging for nutritious young leaves, ripe fruit, and flowers, as well as detecting predator camouflage and emotional states in other primates.{{cite journal | vauthors = Vorobyev M | title = Ecology and evolution of primate colour vision | journal = Clinical & Experimental Optometry | volume = 87 | issue = 4–5 | pages = 230–8 | date = July 2004 | pmid = 15312027 | doi = 10.1111/j.1444-0938.2004.tb05053.x | s2cid = 40234800 | doi-access = free }}{{cite journal | vauthors = Carvalho LS, Pessoa D, Mountford JK, Davies WI, Hunt DM |title=The Genetic and Evolutionary Drives behind Primate Color Vision |journal=Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution |date=26 April 2017 |volume=5 |doi=10.3389/fevo.2017.00034|doi-access=free }}{{cite journal | vauthors = Hiramatsu C, Melin AD, Allen WL, Dubuc C, Higham JP | title = Experimental evidence that primate trichromacy is well suited for detecting primate social colour signals | journal = Proceedings. Biological Sciences | volume = 284 | issue = 1856 | pages = 20162458 | date = June 2017 | pmid = 28615496 | pmc = 5474062 | doi = 10.1098/rspb.2016.2458 }}

Wavelength

{{Spectral colors simple table}}

Isaac Newton discovered that white light after being split into its component colors when passed through a dispersive prism could be recombined to make white light by passing them through a different prism.File:Eyesensitivity.svg relative brightness sensitivity of the human visual system as a function of wavelength (luminosity function)]] The visible light spectrum ranges from about 380 to 740 nanometers. Spectral colors (colors that are produced by a narrow band of wavelengths) such as red, orange, yellow, green, cyan, blue, and violet can be found in this range. These spectral colors do not refer to a single wavelength, but rather to a set of wavelengths: red, 625–740 nm; orange, 590–625 nm; yellow, 565–590 nm; green, 500–565 nm; cyan, 485–500 nm; blue, 450–485 nm; violet, 380–450 nm.

Wavelengths longer or shorter than this range are called infrared or ultraviolet, respectively. Humans cannot generally see these wavelengths, but other animals may.

= Hue detection =

Sufficient differences in wavelength cause a difference in the perceived hue; the just-noticeable difference in wavelength varies from about 1 nm in the blue-green and yellow wavelengths to 10 nm and more in the longer red and shorter blue wavelengths. Although the human eye can distinguish up to a few hundred hues, when those pure spectral colors are mixed together or diluted with white light, the number of distinguishable chromaticities can be much higher.

In very low light levels, vision is scotopic: light is detected by rod cells of the retina. Rods are maximally sensitive to wavelengths near 500 nm and play little, if any, role in color vision. In brighter light, such as daylight, vision is photopic: light is detected by cone cells which are responsible for color vision. Cones are sensitive to a range of wavelengths, but are most sensitive to wavelengths near 555 nm. Between these regions, mesopic vision comes into play and both rods and cones provide signals to the retinal ganglion cells. The shift in color perception from dim light to daylight gives rise to differences known as the Purkinje effect.

The perception of "white" is formed by the entire spectrum of visible light, or by mixing colors of just a few wavelengths in animals with few types of color receptors. In humans, white light can be perceived by combining wavelengths such as red, green, and blue, or just a pair of complementary colors such as blue and yellow.{{cite encyclopedia | vauthors = Davson H, Perkins ES | title = Human eye | url = https://www.britannica.com/science/human-eye | encyclopedia = Encyclopedia Britannica | date = 7 August 2020 }}

= Non-spectral colors =

There are a variety of colors in addition to spectral colors and their hues. These include grayscale colors, shades of colors obtained by mixing grayscale colors with spectral colors, violet-red colors, impossible colors, and metallic colors.

Grayscale colors include white, gray, and black. Rods contain rhodopsin, which reacts to light intensity, providing grayscale coloring.

Shades include colors such as pink or brown. Pink is obtained from mixing red and white. Brown may be obtained from mixing orange with gray or black. Navy is obtained from mixing blue and black.

Violet-red colors include hues and shades of magenta. The light spectrum is a line on which violet is one end and the other is red, and yet we see hues of purple that connect those two colors.

Impossible colors are a combination of cone responses that cannot be naturally produced. For example, medium cones cannot be activated completely on their own; if they were, we would see a 'hyper-green' color.

Dimensionality

Color vision is categorized foremost according to the dimensionality of the color gamut, which is defined by the number of primaries required to represent the color vision. This is generally equal to the number of photopsins expressed: a correlation that holds for vertebrates but not invertebrates. The common vertebrate ancestor possessed four photopsins (expressed in cones) plus rhodopsin (expressed in rods), so was tetrachromatic. However, many vertebrate lineages have lost one or many photopsin genes, leading to lower-dimension color vision. The dimensions of color vision range from 1-dimensional and up:

class="wikitable nowrap"

!Dimension

!Characteristic

!Occurrence

Monochromacy

|1D color vision
lack of any color perception

|Some mammals, including Pinnipeds, Cetaceans and Xenarthra{{cite journal|last1=Peichl|first1=Leo|last2=Behrmann |first2=Gunther |last3=Kroger |first3=Ronald H. H.|title=For whales and seals the ocean is not blue: a visual pigment loss in marine mammals|journal=European Journal of Neuroscience|date=April 2001|volume=13|issue=8|pages=1520–1528|doi=10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01533.x|pmid=11328346|citeseerx=10.1.1.486.616|s2cid=16062564}}{{Cite journal|last1=Emerling|first1=Christopher A.|last2=Springer|first2=Mark S.|date=2015-02-07|title=Genomic evidence for rod monochromacy in sloths and armadillos suggests early subterranean history for Xenarthra|journal=Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences|volume=282|issue=1800|pages=20142192|doi=10.1098/rspb.2014.2192|issn=0962-8452|pmc=4298209|pmid=25540280}}

Dichromacy

|2D color vision

|Most mammals and a quarter of color blind humans

Trichromacy

|3D color vision

|Most Old World monkeys and apes, including most humans; possibly monotremes and some marsupials

Tetrachromacy

|4D color vision

|Most birds, reptiles and fish, and rarely in humans

Pentachromacy and higher

|5D+ color vision

|Rare in vertebrates

Physiology of color perception

File:Cone-fundamentals-with-srgb-spectrum.svg

File:Spectrum locus 12.pngPerception of color begins with specialized retinal cells known as cone cells. Cone cells contain different forms of opsin – a pigment protein – that have different spectral sensitivities. Humans contain three types, resulting in trichromatic color vision.

Each individual cone contains pigments composed of opsin apoprotein covalently linked to a light-absorbing prosthetic group: either 11-cis-hydroretinal or, more rarely, 11-cis-dehydroretinal.{{cite journal | vauthors = Nathans J, Thomas D, Hogness DS | title = Molecular genetics of human color vision: the genes encoding blue, green, and red pigments | journal = Science | volume = 232 | issue = 4747 | pages = 193–202 | date = April 1986 | pmid = 2937147 | doi = 10.1126/science.2937147 | bibcode = 1986Sci...232..193N | s2cid = 34321827 | jstor = 169687 }}

The cones are conventionally labeled according to the ordering of the wavelengths of the peaks of their spectral sensitivities: short (S), medium (M), and long (L) cone types. These three types do not correspond well to particular colors as we know them. Rather, the perception of color is achieved by a complex process that starts with the differential output of these cells in the retina and which is finalized in the visual cortex and associative areas of the brain.

For example, while the L cones have been referred to simply as red receptors, microspectrophotometry has shown that their peak sensitivity is in the greenish-yellow region of the spectrum. Similarly, the S cones and M cones do not directly correspond to blue and green, although they are often described as such. The RGB color model, therefore, is a convenient means for representing color but is not directly based on the types of cones in the human eye.

The peak response of human cone cells varies, even among individuals with so-called normal color vision;{{cite journal | vauthors = Neitz J, Jacobs GH | title = Polymorphism of the long-wavelength cone in normal human colour vision | journal = Nature | volume = 323 | issue = 6089 | pages = 623–5 | year = 1986 | pmid = 3773989 | doi = 10.1038/323623a0 | s2cid = 4316301 | bibcode = 1986Natur.323..623N }}

in some non-human species this polymorphic variation is even greater, and it may well be adaptive.{{technical inline |date=May 2018 | Cited source doesn't mention adaptive color responses or sensitivity in the publically–available summary. I can guess what “adaptive” describes in this context, but I could be incorrect, and therefore cannot clarify: it could be photoreceptors which shift their sensitivity, or it could be visual processing which sees some colors more or less strongly. }}{{cite journal | vauthors = Jacobs GH | title = Primate photopigments and primate color vision | journal = Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America | volume = 93 | issue = 2 | pages = 577–81 | date = January 1996 | pmid = 8570598 | pmc = 40094 | doi = 10.1073/pnas.93.2.577 | bibcode = 1996PNAS...93..577J | doi-access = free }}

=Theories=

File:Diagram of the opponent process.png

Two complementary theories of color vision are the trichromatic theory and the opponent process theory. The trichromatic theory, or Young–Helmholtz theory, proposed in the 19th century by Thomas Young and Hermann von Helmholtz, posits three types of cones preferentially sensitive to blue, green, and red, respectively. Others have suggested that the trichromatic theory is not specifically a theory of color vision but a theory of receptors for all vision, including color but not specific or limited to it.{{Cite journal |last=Zeki |first=Semir |date=2022-10-09 |title=The Paton prize lecture 2021: A colourful experience leading to a reassessment of colour vision and its theories |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1113/ep089760 |journal=Experimental Physiology |volume=107 |issue=11 |pages=1189–1208 |doi=10.1113/ep089760 |pmid=36114718 |s2cid=252335063 |issn=0958-0670}} Equally, it has been suggested that the relationship between the phenomenal opponency described by Ewald Hering and the physiological opponent processes are not straightforward (see below), making of physiological opponency a mechanism that is relevant to the whole of vision, and not just to color vision alone. Hering proposed the opponent process theory in 1872.{{cite journal|title=Zur Lehre vom Lichtsinne |journal=Sitzungsberichte der Mathematisch–Naturwissenschaftliche Classe der Kaiserlichen Akademie der Wissenschaften |issue=III Abtheilung |volume=LXVI. Band | vauthors = Hering E |author-link=Ewald Hering |year=1872 | url=https://books.google.com/books?id=u5MCAAAAYAAJ&q=1872+hering+ewald+Zur+Lehre+vom+Lichtsinne.+Sitzungsberichte+der+kaiserlichen+Akademie+der+Wissenschaften.+Mathematisch%E2%80%93naturwissenschaftliche+Classe,&pg=PA5 |publisher=K.-K. Hof- und Staatsdruckerei in Commission bei C. Gerold's Sohn}} It states that the visual system interprets color in an antagonistic way: red vs. green, blue vs. yellow, black vs. white. Both theories are generally accepted as valid, describing different stages in visual physiology, visualized in the adjacent diagram.{{rp|168}}

Green–magenta and blue–yellow are scales with mutually exclusive boundaries. In the same way that there cannot exist a "slightly negative" positive number, a single eye cannot perceive a bluish-yellow or a reddish-green. Although these two theories are both currently widely accepted theories, past and more recent work has led to criticism of the opponent process theory, stemming from a number of what are presented as discrepancies in the standard opponent process theory. For example, the phenomenon of an after-image of complementary color can be induced by fatiguing the cells responsible for color perception, by staring at a vibrant color for a length of time, and then looking at a white surface. This phenomenon of complementary colors shows that cyan, rather than green, is the complement of red, and that magenta, rather than red, is the complement of green. It therefore also shows that the reddish-green color supposed to be impossible by opponent process theory is actually the color yellow. Although this phenomenon is more readily explained by the trichromatic theory, explanations for the discrepancy may include alterations to the opponent process theory, such as redefining the opponent colors as red vs. cyan, to reflect this effect. Despite such criticisms, both theories remain in use.

A newer theory proposed by Edwin H. Land, the Retinex Theory, is based on a demonstration of color constancy, which shows that the color of any surface that is part of a complex natural scene is to a large degree independent of the wavelength composition of the light reflected from it. Also the after-image produced by looking at a given part of a complex scene is also independent of the wavelength composition of the light reflected from it alone. Thus, while the color of the after-image produced by looking at a green surface that is reflecting more "green" (middle-wave) than "red" (long-wave) light is magenta, so is the after–image of the same surface when it reflects more "red" than "green" light (when it is still perceived as green). This would seem to rule out an explanation of color opponency based on retinal cone adaptation.{{cite journal | vauthors = Zeki S, Cheadle S, Pepper J, Mylonas D | title = The Constancy of Colored After-Images | language = English | journal = Frontiers in Human Neuroscience | volume = 11 | pages = 229 | date = 2017 | pmid = 28539878 | pmc = 5423953 | doi = 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00229 | doi-access = free }} 50px Text was copied from this source, which is available under a [https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License].

According to Land's Retinex theory, color in a natural scene depends upon the three sets of cone cells ("red," "green," and "blue") separately perceiving each surface's relative lightness in the scene and, together with the visual cortex, assigning color based on comparing the lightness values perceived by each set of cone cells.{{Cite journal|url=https://www.jstor.org/stable/24953876|title=The Retinex Theory of Color Vision|last=Land|first=Edwin|date=December 1977|journal=Scientific American|pmid=929159|doi=10.1038/scientificamerican1277-108|volume=237|issue=6|pages=108–28|jstor=24953876 |bibcode=1977SciAm.237f.108L}}

=Cone cells in the human eye=

File:Distribution of Cones and Rods on Human Retina.png

A range of wavelengths of light stimulates each of these receptor types to varying degrees. The brain combines the information from each type of receptor to give rise to different perceptions of different wavelengths of light.

class="wikitable"

!Cone type

NameRangePeak wavelength{{cite book | vauthors = Wyszecki G, Stiles WS | year = 1982 | title = Color Science: Concepts and Methods, Quantitative Data and Formulae | edition = 2nd | publisher = Wiley Series in Pure and Applied Optics | location = New York | isbn = 978-0-471-02106-3 }}{{cite book | vauthors = Hunt RW | year = 2004 | title = The Reproduction of Colour | edition = 6th | pages = [https://archive.org/details/reproductionofco0000hunt/page/11 11–2] | publisher = Wiley–IS&T Series in Imaging Science and Technology | location = Chichester UK | isbn = 978-0-470-02425-6 | url = https://archive.org/details/reproductionofco0000hunt/page/11 }}
Sβ400–500 nm420–440 nm
Mγ450–630 nm534–555 nm
Lρ500–700 nm564–580 nm

Cones and rods are not evenly distributed in the human eye. Cones have a high density at the fovea and a low density in the rest of the retina.{{cite journal| vauthors = Purves D, Augustine GJ, Fitzpatrick D, Katz LC, LaMantia AS, McNamara JO, Williams SM |date=2001|title=Anatomical Distribution of Rods and Cones|url=https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK10848/|journal=Neuroscience. 2nd Edition|language=en}} Thus color information is mostly taken in at the fovea. Humans have poor color perception in their peripheral vision, and much of the color we see in our periphery may be filled in by what our brains expect to be there on the basis of context and memories. However, our accuracy of color perception in the periphery increases with the size of stimulus.{{cite journal | vauthors = Johnson MA | title = Color vision in the peripheral retina | journal = American Journal of Optometry and Physiological Optics | volume = 63 | issue = 2 | pages = 97–103 | date = February 1986 | pmid = 3953765 | doi = 10.1097/00006324-198602000-00003 }}

The opsins (photopigments) present in the L and M cones are encoded on the X chromosome; defective encoding of these leads to the two most common forms of color blindness. The OPN1LW gene, which encodes the opsin present in the L cones, is highly polymorphic; one study found 85 variants in a sample of 236 men.{{cite journal | vauthors = Verrelli BC, Tishkoff SA | title = Signatures of selection and gene conversion associated with human color vision variation | journal = American Journal of Human Genetics | volume = 75 | issue = 3 | pages = 363–75 | date = September 2004 | pmid = 15252758 | pmc = 1182016 | doi = 10.1086/423287 }} A small percentage of women may have an extra type of color receptor because they have different alleles for the gene for the L opsin on each X chromosome. X chromosome inactivation means that while only one opsin is expressed in each cone cell, both types may occur overall, and some women may therefore show a degree of tetrachromatic color vision.{{cite web | vauthors = Roth M | date = 2006 | url = http://www.post-gazette.com/pg/06256/721190-114.stm | title = Some women may see 100 million colors, thanks to their genes | archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20061108171635/http://www.post-gazette.com/pg/06256/721190-114.stm | archive-date=2006-11-08 | work = Post-Gazette.com }} Variations in OPN1MW, which encodes the opsin expressed in M cones, appear to be rare, and the observed variants have no effect on spectral sensitivity.

=Color in the primate brain=

File:Ventral-dorsal streams.svg (purple) is important in color recognition. The dorsal stream (green) is also shown. They originate from a common source in the visual cortex.]]

Color processing begins at a very early level in the visual system (even within the retina) through initial color opponent mechanisms. Both Helmholtz's trichromatic theory and Hering's opponent-process theory are therefore correct, but trichromacy arises at the level of the receptors, and opponent processes arise at the level of retinal ganglion cells and beyond. In Hering's theory, opponent mechanisms refer to the opposing color effect of red–green, blue–yellow, and light-dark. However, in the visual system, it is the activity of the different receptor types that are opposed. Some midget retinal ganglion cells oppose L and M cone activity, which corresponds loosely to red–green opponency, but actually runs along an axis from blue-green to magenta. Small bistratified retinal ganglion cells oppose input from the S cones to input from the L and M cones. This is often thought to correspond to blue–yellow opponency but actually runs along a color axis from yellow-green to violet.

Visual information is then sent to the brain from retinal ganglion cells via the optic nerve to the optic chiasma: a point where the two optic nerves meet and information from the temporal (contralateral) visual field crosses to the other side of the brain. After the optic chiasma, the visual tracts are referred to as the optic tracts, which enter the thalamus to synapse at the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN).

The lateral geniculate nucleus is divided into laminae (zones), of which there are three types: the M-laminae, consisting primarily of M-cells, the P-laminae, consisting primarily of P-cells, and the koniocellular laminae. M- and P-cells receive relatively balanced input from both L- and M-cones throughout most of the retina, although this seems to not be the case at the fovea, with midget cells synapsing in the P-laminae. The koniocellular laminae receives axons from the small bistratified ganglion cells.{{cite book | vauthors = Rodieck RW | title = The First Steps in Seeing | publisher = Sinauer Associates, Inc. | location = Sunderland, Massachusetts, USA | date = 1998 | isbn = 978-0-87893-757-8 }}{{cite journal | vauthors = Hendry SH, Reid RC | title = The koniocellular pathway in primate vision | journal = Annual Review of Neuroscience | volume = 23 | pages = 127–53 | date = 1970-01-01 | pmid = 10845061 | doi = 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.127 }}

After synapsing at the LGN, the visual tract continues on back to the primary visual cortex (V1) located at the back of the brain within the occipital lobe. Within V1 there is a distinct band (striation). This is also referred to as "striate cortex", with other cortical visual regions referred to collectively as "extrastriate cortex". It is at this stage that color processing becomes much more complicated.

In V1 the simple three-color segregation begins to break down. Many cells in V1 respond to some parts of the spectrum better than others, but this "color tuning" is often different depending on the adaptation state of the visual system. A given cell that might respond best to long-wavelength light if the light is relatively bright might then become responsive to all wavelengths if the stimulus is relatively dim. Because the color tuning of these cells is not stable, some believe that a different, relatively small, population of neurons in V1 is responsible for color vision. These specialized "color cells" often have receptive fields that can compute local cone ratios. Such "double-opponent" cells were initially described in the goldfish retina by Nigel Daw;{{cite journal | vauthors = Daw NW | title = Goldfish retina: organization for simultaneous color contrast | journal = Science | volume = 158 | issue = 3803 | pages = 942–4 | date = November 1967 | pmid = 6054169 | doi = 10.1126/science.158.3803.942 | s2cid = 1108881 | bibcode = 1967Sci...158..942D }}{{cite book| title = Neural Mechanisms of Color Vision: Double-Opponent Cells in the Visual Cortex | vauthors = Conway BR | url = https://books.google.com/books?id=pFodUlHfQmcC&q=goldfish+retina+by+Nigel-Daw&pg=PR7 | publisher = Springer | year = 2002 | isbn = 978-1-4020-7092-1 }} their existence in primates was suggested by David H. Hubel and Torsten Wiesel, first demonstrated by C.R. Michael{{Cite journal|last=Michael|first=C. R.|date=1978-05-01|title=Color vision mechanisms in monkey striate cortex: dual-opponent cells with concentric receptive fields|url=https://journals.physiology.org/doi/abs/10.1152/jn.1978.41.3.572|journal=Journal of Neurophysiology|volume=41|issue=3|pages=572–588|doi=10.1152/jn.1978.41.3.572|pmid=96222|issn=0022-3077}} and subsequently confirmed by Bevil Conway.{{cite journal | vauthors = Conway BR | title = Spatial structure of cone inputs to color cells in alert macaque primary visual cortex (V-1) | journal = The Journal of Neuroscience | volume = 21 | issue = 8 | pages = 2768–83 | date = April 2001 | pmid = 11306629 | pmc = 6762533 | doi = 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-08-02768.2001 }} As Margaret Livingstone and David Hubel showed, double opponent cells are clustered within localized regions of V1 called blobs, and are thought to come in two flavors, red–green and blue-yellow.{{cite book| title = Neurons, and Networks: An Introduction to Behavioral Neuroscience | vauthors = Dowling JE | publisher = Harvard University Press | year = 2001 | isbn = 978-0-674-00462-7 | url = https://books.google.com/books?id=adeUwgfwdKwC&q=Margaret+Livingstone+David+Hubel+double+opponent+blobs&pg=PA376 }} Red–green cells compare the relative amounts of red–green in one part of a scene with the amount of red–green in an adjacent part of the scene, responding best to local color contrast (red next to green). Modeling studies have shown that double-opponent cells are ideal candidates for the neural machinery of color constancy explained by Edwin H. Land in his retinex theory.{{cite book | veditors = McCann M | date = 1993 | title = Edwin H. Land's Essays | location = Springfield, Va. | publisher = Society for Imaging Science and Technology }}

File:16777216colors.png can distinguish about 10 million different colors.{{cite book | vauthors = Judd DB, Wyszecki G |title=Color in Business, Science and Industry|publisher=Wiley-Interscience|series=Wiley Series in Pure and Applied Optics|edition=third|location=New York|year= 1975|page=388|isbn=978-0-471-45212-6}}]]

From the V1 blobs, color information is sent to cells in the second visual area, V2. The cells in V2 that are most strongly color tuned are clustered in the "thin stripes" that, like the blobs in V1, stain for the enzyme cytochrome oxidase (separating the thin stripes are interstripes and thick stripes, which seem to be concerned with other visual information like motion and high-resolution form). Neurons in V2 then synapse onto cells in the extended V4. This area includes not only V4, but two other areas in the posterior inferior temporal cortex, anterior to area V3, the dorsal posterior inferior temporal cortex, and posterior TEO.{{cite journal | vauthors = Conway BR, Moeller S, Tsao DY | title = Specialized color modules in macaque extrastriate cortex | journal = Neuron | volume = 56 | issue = 3 | pages = 560–73 | date = November 2007 | pmid = 17988638 | doi = 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.008 | pmc = 8162777 | s2cid = 11724926 | url = https://authors.library.caltech.edu/100800/ }}{{cite journal | vauthors = Conway BR, Tsao DY | title = Color-tuned neurons are spatially clustered according to color preference within alert macaque posterior inferior temporal cortex | journal = Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America | volume = 106 | issue = 42 | pages = 18034–9 | date = October 2009 | pmid = 19805195 | pmc = 2764907 | doi = 10.1073/pnas.0810943106 | bibcode = 2009PNAS..10618034C | doi-access = free }} Area V4 was initially suggested by Semir Zeki to be exclusively dedicated to color,{{cite journal | vauthors = Zeki SM | title = Colour coding in rhesus monkey prestriate cortex | journal = Brain Research | volume = 53 | issue = 2 | pages = 422–7 | date = April 1973 | pmid = 4196224 | doi = 10.1016/0006-8993(73)90227-8 }} and he later showed that V4 can be subdivided into subregions with very high concentrations of color cells separated from each other by zones with lower concentration of such cells though even the latter cells respond better to some wavelengths than to others,{{cite journal | vauthors = Zeki S | title = The distribution of wavelength and orientation selective cells in different areas of monkey visual cortex | journal = Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences | volume = 217 | issue = 1209 | pages = 449–70 | date = March 1983 | pmid = 6134287 | doi = 10.1098/rspb.1983.0020 | bibcode = 1983RSPSB.217..449Z | s2cid = 39700958 }} a finding confirmed by subsequent studies.{{cite journal | vauthors = Bushnell BN, Harding PJ, Kosai Y, Bair W, Pasupathy A | title = Equiluminance cells in visual cortical area v4 | journal = The Journal of Neuroscience | volume = 31 | issue = 35 | pages = 12398–412 | date = August 2011 | pmid = 21880901 | pmc = 3171995 | doi = 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1890-11.2011 }}{{cite journal | vauthors = Tanigawa H, Lu HD, Roe AW | title = Functional organization for color and orientation in macaque V4 | journal = Nature Neuroscience | volume = 13 | issue = 12 | pages = 1542–8 | date = December 2010 | pmid = 21076422 | pmc = 3005205 | doi = 10.1038/nn.2676 }} The presence in V4 of orientation-selective cells led to the view that V4 is involved in processing both color and form associated with color{{cite journal | vauthors = Zeki S | title = The Ferrier Lecture 1995 behind the seen: the functional specialization of the brain in space and time | journal = Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences | volume = 360 | issue = 1458 | pages = 1145–83 | date = June 2005 | pmid = 16147515 | pmc = 1609195 | doi = 10.1098/rstb.2005.1666 }} but it is worth noting that the orientation selective cells within V4 are more broadly tuned than their counterparts in V1, V2 and V3. Color processing in the extended V4 occurs in millimeter-sized color modules called globs. This is the part of the brain in which color is first processed into the full range of hues found in color space.{{Cite journal|last=Zeki|first=S.|date=1980|title=The representation of colours in the cerebral cortex|url=https://www.nature.com/articles/284412a0|journal=Nature|language=en|volume=284|issue=5755|pages=412–418|doi=10.1038/284412a0|pmid=6767195|bibcode=1980Natur.284..412Z |s2cid=4310049|issn=1476-4687}}

Anatomical studies have shown that neurons in extended V4 provide input to the inferior temporal lobe. "IT" cortex is thought to integrate color information with shape and form, although it has been difficult to define the appropriate criteria for this claim. Despite this murkiness, it has been useful to characterize this pathway (V1 > V2 > V4 > IT) as the ventral stream or the "what pathway", distinguished from the dorsal stream ("where pathway") that is thought to analyze motion, among other features.

Subjectivity of color perception <span class="anchor" id="Subjectivity"></span>

{{further|Color appearance}}

{{See also|Linguistic relativity and the color naming debate}}

Color is a feature of visual perception by an observer. There is a complex relationship between the wavelengths of light in the visual spectrum and human experiences of color. Although most people are assumed to have the same mapping, the philosopher John Locke recognized that alternatives are possible, and described one such hypothetical case with the "inverted spectrum" thought experiment. For example, someone with an inverted spectrum might experience green while seeing 'red' (700 nm) light, and experience red while seeing 'green' (530 nm) light. This inversion has never been demonstrated in experiment, though.

Synesthesia (or ideasthesia) provides some atypical but illuminating examples of subjective color experience triggered by input that is not even light, such as sounds or shapes. The possibility of a clean dissociation between color experience from properties of the world reveals that color is a subjective psychological phenomenon.

The Himba people have been found to categorize colors differently from most Westerners and are able to easily distinguish close shades of green, barely discernible for most people.{{cite book | vauthors = Roberson D, Davidoff J, Davies IR, Shapiro LR | chapter = Colour categories and category acquisition in Himba and English | chapter-url = https://eclass.uoa.gr/modules/document/file.php/ECD246/Διάφορα%20κείμενα%20και%20για%20εργασίες/ProgressInColour.pdf | veditors = Pitchford N, Biggam CP | title = Progress in Colour Studies | volume = II. Psychological aspects | date = November 2006 | pages = 159–72 | isbn = 978-90-272-9302-2 | publisher = John Benjamins Publishing }} The Himba have created a very different color scheme which divides the spectrum to dark shades (zuzu in Himba), very light (vapa), vivid blue and green (buru) and dry colors as an adaptation to their specific way of life.

The perception of color depends heavily on the context in which the perceived object is presented.{{Cite book |last=Nänni |first=Jürg |title=Visual perception - an interactive journey of discovery through our visual system |publisher=Niggli AG |year=2008 |isbn=978-3-7212-0618-0 |location=Sulgen Zurich, Switzerland |language=German, English}}

Psychophysical experiments have shown that color is perceived before the orientation of lines and directional motion by as much as 40ms and 80 ms respectively, thus leading to a perceptual asynchrony that is demonstrable with brief presentation times.

= Chromatic adaptation =

{{Main|Chromatic adaptation}}

In color vision, chromatic adaptation refers to color constancy; the ability of the visual system to preserve the appearance of an object under a wide range of light sources.{{cite book| vauthors = Fairchild MD |title=Color Appearance Models|publisher=Wiley|year=2005|isbn=978-0-470-01216-1|page=146|chapter=8. Chromatic Adaptation|chapter-url=https://books.google.com/books?id=8_TxzK2B-5MC&q=%22chromatic+adaptation%22&pg=PA146}} For example, a white page under blue, pink, or purple light will reflect mostly blue, pink, or purple light to the eye, respectively; the brain, however, compensates for the effect of lighting (based on the color shift of surrounding objects) and is more likely to interpret the page as white under all three conditions, a phenomenon known as color constancy.

In color science, chromatic adaptation is the estimation of the representation of an object under a different light source from the one in which it was recorded. A common application is to find a chromatic adaptation transform (CAT) that will make the recording of a neutral object appear neutral (color balance), while keeping other colors also looking realistic.{{cite web | vauthors = Süsstrunk S | url = http://ivrgwww.epfl.ch/research/past_topics/chromatic_adaptation.html | title = Chromatic Adaptation | archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20110818184220/http://ivrgwww.epfl.ch/research/past_topics/chromatic_adaptation.html| archive-date = 2011-08-18 | work = The Image and Visual Representation Lab (IVRL) }} For example, chromatic adaptation transforms are used when converting images between ICC profiles with different white points. Adobe Photoshop, for example, uses the Bradford CAT.{{cite web | vauthors = Lindbloom B | url = http://www.brucelindbloom.com/Eqn_ChromAdapt.html | title = Chromatic Adaptation | archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20110926223613/http://www.brucelindbloom.com/Eqn_ChromAdapt.html| archive-date=2011-09-26 | work = Lindbloom.com }}

Color vision in nonhumans

Many species can see light with frequencies outside the human "visible spectrum". Bees and many other insects can detect ultraviolet light,{{Cite book |title=World Book |date=2022 |publisher=World Book, Inc. |isbn=9780716601227 |edition=72nd |location=Chicago, IL |publication-date=2022 |pages=819}} which helps them to find nectar in flowers. Plant species that depend on insect pollination may owe reproductive success to ultraviolet "colors" and patterns rather than how colorful they appear to humans. Birds, too, can see into the ultraviolet (300–400 nm), and some have sex-dependent markings on their plumage that are visible only in the ultraviolet range.{{cite book| vauthors = Cuthill IC |author-link=Innes Cuthill | veditors = Slater PJ |title=Advances in the Study of Behavior|publisher=Academic Press|location=Oxford, England|year=1997|volume=29|chapter=Ultraviolet vision in birds|page=161|isbn=978-0-12-004529-7}}{{cite book| vauthors = Jamieson BG |title=Reproductive Biology and Phylogeny of Birds|publisher=University of Virginia|location=Charlottesville VA|year=2007|page=128|isbn=978-1-57808-386-2}} Many animals that can see into the ultraviolet range, however, cannot see red light or any other reddish wavelengths. For example, bees' visible spectrum ends at about 590 nm, just before the orange wavelengths start. Birds, however, can see some red wavelengths, although not as far into the light spectrum as humans.{{cite book | vauthors = Varela FJ, Palacios AG, Goldsmith TH | chapter = Color vision of birds | pages = 77–94 | chapter-url = https://books.google.com/books?id=p1SUzc5GUVcC | veditors = Zeigler HP, Bischof HJ | publisher = MIT Press | date = 1993 | title = Vision, Brain, and Behavior in Birds |isbn=978-0-262-24036-9}} It is a myth that the common goldfish is the only animal that can see both infrared and ultraviolet light;{{cite web |url= http://www.skeptive.com/disputes/4484 |title=True or False? The common goldfish is the only animal that can see both infrared and ultra-violet light. | work = Skeptive |access-date=September 28, 2013 |url-status=usurped |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20131224110616/http://www.skeptive.com/disputes/4484 |archive-date=December 24, 2013 }} their color vision extends into the ultraviolet but not the infrared.{{cite book | vauthors = Neumeyer C | veditors = Lazareva O, Shimizu T, Wasserman E |title= How Animals See the World: Comparative Behavior, Biology, and Evolution of Vision |publisher=Oxford Scholarship Online |year=2012 |chapter=Chapter 2: Color Vision in Goldfish and Other Vertebrates |isbn= 978-0-195-33465-4}}

The basis for this variation is the number of cone types that differ between species. Mammals, in general, have a color vision of a limited type, and usually have red–green color blindness, with only two types of cones. Humans, some primates, and some marsupials see an extended range of colors, but only by comparison with other mammals. Most non-mammalian vertebrate species distinguish different colors at least as well as humans, and many species of birds, fish, reptiles, and amphibians, and some invertebrates, have more than three cone types and probably superior color vision to humans.

In most Catarrhini (Old World monkeys and apes—primates closely related to humans), there are three types of color receptors (known as cone cells), resulting in trichromatic color vision. These primates, like humans, are known as trichromats. Many other primates (including New World monkeys) and other mammals are dichromats, which is the general color vision state for mammals that are active during the day (i.e., felines, canines, ungulates). Nocturnal mammals may have little or no color vision. Trichromat non-primate mammals are rare.{{cite book| vauthors = Ali MA, Klyne MA |title=Vision in Vertebrates|place=New York|publisher=Plenum Press|year=1985 |isbn=978-0-306-42065-8}}{{rp|174–175}}{{cite journal | vauthors = Jacobs GH | title = The distribution and nature of colour vision among the mammals | journal = Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society | volume = 68 | issue = 3 | pages = 413–71 | date = August 1993 | pmid = 8347768 | doi = 10.1111/j.1469-185X.1993.tb00738.x | s2cid = 24172719 }}

Many invertebrates have color vision. Honeybees and bumblebees have trichromatic color vision which is insensitive to red but sensitive to ultraviolet. Osmia rufa, for example, possess a trichromatic color system, which they use in foraging for pollen from flowers.{{cite journal|title = Spectral Sensitivity of Photoreceptors and Colour Vision in the Solitary Bee, Osmia Rufa|url = http://jeb.biologists.org/content/136/1/35|journal = Journal of Experimental Biology|date = 1988-05-01|issn = 0022-0949|pages = 35–52|volume = 136|issue = 1| vauthors = Menzel R, Steinmann E, De Souza J, Backhaus W |doi = 10.1242/jeb.136.1.35|url-status = live|archive-url = https://web.archive.org/web/20160304133039/http://jeb.biologists.org/content/136/1/35|archive-date = 2016-03-04}} In view of the importance of color vision to bees one might expect these receptor sensitivities to reflect their specific visual ecology; for example the types of flowers that they visit. However, the main groups of hymenopteran insects excluding ants (i.e., bees, wasps and sawflies) mostly have three types of photoreceptor, with spectral sensitivities similar to the honeybee's.{{cite journal | vauthors = Osorio D, Vorobyev M | title = A review of the evolution of animal colour vision and visual communication signals | journal = Vision Research | volume = 48 | issue = 20 | pages = 2042–51 | date = September 2008 | pmid = 18627773 | doi = 10.1016/j.visres.2008.06.018 | s2cid = 12025276 | doi-access = free }} Papilio butterflies possess six types of photoreceptors and may have pentachromatic vision.{{cite journal | vauthors = Arikawa K | title = Spectral organization of the eye of a butterfly, Papilio | journal = Journal of Comparative Physiology A: Neuroethology, Sensory, Neural & Behavioral Physiology | volume = 189 | issue = 11 | pages = 791–800 | date = November 2003 | pmid = 14520495 | doi = 10.1007/s00359-003-0454-7 | s2cid = 25685593 }} The most complex color vision system in the animal kingdom has been found in stomatopods (such as the mantis shrimp) having between 12 and 16 spectral receptor types thought to work as multiple dichromatic units.{{cite journal |vauthors=Cronin TW, Marshall NJ |title=A retina with at least ten spectral types of photoreceptors in a mantis shrimp |journal=Nature |volume=339 |pages=137–40 |year=1989 |doi=10.1038/339137a0 |issue=6220 |bibcode=1989Natur.339..137C |s2cid=4367079 }}

Vertebrate animals such as tropical fish and birds sometimes have more complex color vision systems than humans; thus the many subtle colors they exhibit generally serve as direct signals for other fish or birds, and not to signal mammals.{{cite journal | vauthors = Kelber A, Vorobyev M, Osorio D | title = Animal colour vision--behavioural tests and physiological concepts | journal = Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society | volume = 78 | issue = 1 | pages = 81–118 | date = February 2003 | pmid = 12620062 | doi = 10.1017/S1464793102005985 | s2cid = 7610125 | url = https://lup.lub.lu.se/record/131377 }} In bird vision, tetrachromacy is achieved through up to four cone types, depending on species. Each single cone contains one of the four main types of vertebrate cone photopigment (LWS/ MWS, RH2, SWS2 and SWS1) and has a colored oil droplet in its inner segment. Brightly colored oil droplets inside the cones shift or narrow the spectral sensitivity of the cell. Pigeons may be pentachromats.{{cite book | vauthors = Thompson E | chapter = Introducing Comparative Colour Vision |title=Colour vision : a study in cognitive science and the philosophy of perception |date=1995 |publisher=Routledge |location=London |isbn=978-0-203-41767-6 | page = 149 | chapter-url = https://books.google.com/books?id=fB0madWbjBIC&pg=PA149 }}

Reptiles and amphibians also have four cone types (occasionally five), and probably see at least the same number of colors that humans do, or perhaps more. In addition, some nocturnal geckos and frogs have the capability of seeing color in dim light.{{cite journal | vauthors = Roth LS, Lundström L, Kelber A, Kröger RH, Unsbo P | title = The pupils and optical systems of gecko eyes | journal = Journal of Vision | volume = 9 | issue = 3 | pages = 27.1–11 | date = March 2009 | pmid = 19757966 | doi = 10.1167/9.3.27 | doi-access = free }}{{cite journal | vauthors = Yovanovich CA, Koskela SM, Nevala N, Kondrashev SL, Kelber A, Donner K | title = The dual rod system of amphibians supports colour discrimination at the absolute visual threshold | journal = Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences | volume = 372 | issue = 1717 | date = April 2017 | pmid = 28193811 | pmc = 5312016 | doi = 10.1098/rstb.2016.0066 | doi-access = free }} At least some color-guided behaviors in amphibians have also been shown to be wholly innate, developing even in visually deprived animals.{{cite journal | vauthors = Hunt JE, Bruno JR, Pratt KG | title = An Innate Color Preference Displayed by Xenopus Tadpoles Is Persistent and Requires the Tegmentum | journal = Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience | volume = 14 | issue = 71 | pages = 71 | date = May 12, 2020 | pmid = 32477078 | pmc = 7235192 | doi = 10.3389/fnbeh.2020.00071 | doi-access = free }}

In the evolution of mammals, segments of color vision were lost, then for a few species of primates, regained by gene duplication. Eutherian mammals other than primates (for example, dogs, mammalian farm animals) generally have less-effective two-receptor (dichromatic) color perception systems, which distinguish blue, green, and yellow—but cannot distinguish oranges and reds. There is some evidence that a few mammals, such as cats, have redeveloped the ability to distinguish longer wavelength colors, in at least a limited way, via one-amino-acid mutations in opsin genes.Shozo Yokoyama and F. Bernhard Radlwimmera, "The Molecular Genetics of Red and Green Color Vision in Mammals", Genetics, Vol. 153, 919–932, October 1999. The adaptation to see reds is particularly important for primate mammals, since it leads to the identification of fruits, and also newly sprouting reddish leaves, which are particularly nutritious.

However, even among primates, full color vision differs between New World and Old World monkeys. Old World primates, including monkeys and all apes, have vision similar to humans. New World monkeys may or may not have color sensitivity at this level: in most species, males are dichromats, and about 60% of females are trichromats, but the owl monkeys are cone monochromats, and both sexes of howler monkeys are trichromats.{{cite journal | vauthors = Jacobs GH, Deegan JF | title = Photopigments and colour vision in New World monkeys from the family Atelidae | journal = Proceedings. Biological Sciences | volume = 268 | issue = 1468 | pages = 695–702 | date = April 2001 | pmid = 11321057 | pmc = 1088658 | doi = 10.1098/rspb.2000.1421 }}{{cite journal | vauthors = Jacobs GH, Deegan JF, Neitz J, Crognale MA, Neitz M | title = Photopigments and color vision in the nocturnal monkey, Aotus | journal = Vision Research | volume = 33 | issue = 13 | pages = 1773–83 | date = September 1993 | pmid = 8266633 | doi = 10.1016/0042-6989(93)90168-V | s2cid = 3745725 | citeseerx = 10.1.1.568.1560 }}{{cite journal | vauthors = Mollon JD, Bowmaker JK, Jacobs GH | title = Variations of colour vision in a New World primate can be explained by polymorphism of retinal photopigments | journal = Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences | volume = 222 | issue = 1228 | pages = 373–99 | date = September 1984 | pmid = 6149558 | doi = 10.1098/rspb.1984.0071 | s2cid = 24416536 | bibcode = 1984RSPSB.222..373M }}{{cite book | vauthors = Sternberg RJ | date = 2006 | title = Cognitive Psychology | edition = 4th | publisher = Thomson Wadsworth }} Visual sensitivity differences between males and females in a single species is due to the gene for yellow-green sensitive opsin protein (which confers ability to differentiate red from green) residing on the X sex chromosome.

Several marsupials, such as the fat-tailed dunnart (Sminthopsis crassicaudata), have trichromatic color vision.{{cite journal | vauthors = Arrese CA, Beazley LD, Neumeyer C | title = Behavioural evidence for marsupial trichromacy | journal = Current Biology | volume = 16 | issue = 6 | pages = R193-4 | date = March 2006 | pmid = 16546067 | doi = 10.1016/j.cub.2006.02.036 | doi-access = free | bibcode = 2006CBio...16.R193A }}

Marine mammals, adapted for low-light vision, have only a single cone type and are thus monochromats.{{Citation needed|date=September 2010}}

Evolution

{{Main|Evolution of color vision}}

{{see also|Evolution of color vision in primates}}

Color perception mechanisms are highly dependent on evolutionary factors, of which the most prominent is thought to be satisfactory recognition of food sources. In herbivorous primates, color perception is essential for finding proper (immature) leaves. In hummingbirds, particular flower types are often recognized by color as well. On the other hand, nocturnal mammals have less-developed color vision since adequate light is needed for cones to function properly. There is evidence that ultraviolet light plays a part in color perception in many branches of the animal kingdom, especially insects. In general, the optical spectrum encompasses the most common electronic transitions in matter and is therefore the most useful for collecting information about the environment.

The evolution of trichromatic color vision in primates occurred as the ancestors of modern monkeys, apes, and humans switched to diurnal (daytime) activity and began consuming fruits and leaves from flowering plants.{{cite book | vauthors = Steven P |title=How the Mind Works |publisher=Norton |location=New York |year=1997 |page=[https://archive.org/details/howmindworks00pink/page/191 191] |isbn=978-0-393-04535-2 |title-link=How the Mind Works |author-link=Steven Pinker }} Color vision, with UV discrimination, is also present in a number of arthropods—the only terrestrial animals besides the vertebrates to possess this trait.{{cite journal | vauthors = Koyanagi M, Nagata T, Katoh K, Yamashita S, Tokunaga F | title = Molecular evolution of arthropod color vision deduced from multiple opsin genes of jumping spiders | journal = Journal of Molecular Evolution | volume = 66 | issue = 2 | pages = 130–7 | date = February 2008 | pmid = 18217181 | doi = 10.1007/s00239-008-9065-9 | s2cid = 23837628 | bibcode = 2008JMolE..66..130K }}

Some animals can distinguish colors in the ultraviolet spectrum. The UV spectrum falls outside the human visible range, except for some cataract surgery patients.{{cite news | url=https://www.theguardian.com/science/2002/may/30/medicalscience.research | title=Let the light shine in: You don't have to come from another planet to see ultraviolet light | publisher=EducationGuardian.co.uk | vauthors = Hambling D | date=May 30, 2002 | url-status=live | archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20141123170913/http://www.theguardian.com/science/2002/may/30/medicalscience.research | archive-date=November 23, 2014 }} Birds, turtles, lizards, many fish and some rodents have UV receptors in their retinas.{{cite journal | vauthors = Jacobs GH, Neitz J, Deegan JF | title = Retinal receptors in rodents maximally sensitive to ultraviolet light | journal = Nature | volume = 353 | issue = 6345 | pages = 655–6 | date = October 1991 | pmid = 1922382 | doi = 10.1038/353655a0 | s2cid = 4283145 | bibcode = 1991Natur.353..655J }} These animals can see the UV patterns found on flowers and other wildlife that are otherwise invisible to the human eye.

Ultraviolet vision is an especially important adaptation in birds. It allows birds to spot small prey from a distance, navigate, avoid predators, and forage while flying at high speeds. Birds also utilize their broad spectrum vision to recognize other birds, and in sexual selection.{{cite book| vauthors = Varela FJ, Palacios AG, Goldsmith TM | veditors = Bischof HJ, Zeigler HP |title=Vision, brain, and behavior in birds |publisher=MIT Press |location=Cambridge, Mass |year=1993 |pages=77–94 |isbn=978-0-262-24036-9 }}{{cite book|title=Advances in the Study of Behavior | vauthors = Cuthill IC, Partridge JC, Bennett AT, Church SC, Hart NS, Hunt S |chapter= Ultraviolet Vision in Birds |year=2000 |volume=29 |pages=159–214 }}

Mathematics of color perception

{{Unreferenced section|date=May 2016 }}

A "physical color" is a combination of pure spectral colors (in the visible range). In principle there exist infinitely many distinct spectral colors, and so the set of all physical colors may be thought of as an infinite-dimensional vector space (a Hilbert space). This space is typically notated Hcolor. More technically, the space of physical colors may be considered to be the topological cone over the simplex whose vertices are the spectral colors, with white at the centroid of the simplex, black at the apex of the cone, and the monochromatic color associated with any given vertex somewhere along the line from that vertex to the apex depending on its brightness.

An element C of Hcolor is a function from the range of visible wavelengths—considered as an interval of real numbers [Wmin,Wmax]—to the real numbers, assigning to each wavelength w in [Wmin,Wmax] its intensity C(w).

A humanly perceived color may be modeled as three numbers: the extents to which each of the 3 types of cones is stimulated. Thus a humanly perceived color may be thought of as a point in 3-dimensional Euclidean space. We call this space R3color.

Since each wavelength w stimulates each of the 3 types of cone cells to a known extent, these extents may be represented by 3 functions s(w), m(w), l(w) corresponding to the response of the S, M, and L cone cells, respectively.

Finally, since a beam of light can be composed of many different wavelengths, to determine the extent to which a physical color C in Hcolor stimulates each cone cell, we must calculate the integral (with respect to w), over the interval [Wmin,Wmax], of C(ws(w), of C(wm(w), and of C(wl(w). The triple of resulting numbers associates with each physical color C (which is an element in Hcolor) a particular perceived color (which is a single point in R3color). This association is easily seen to be linear. It may also easily be seen that many different elements in the "physical" space Hcolor can all result in the same single perceived color in R3color, so a perceived color is not unique to one physical color.

Thus human color perception is determined by a specific, non-unique linear mapping from the infinite-dimensional Hilbert space Hcolor to the 3-dimensional Euclidean space R3color.

Technically, the image of the (mathematical) cone over the simplex whose vertices are the spectral colors, by this linear mapping, is also a (mathematical) cone in R3color. Moving directly away from the vertex of this cone represents maintaining the same chromaticity while increasing its intensity. Taking a cross-section of this cone yields a 2D chromaticity space. Both the 3D cone and its projection or cross-section are convex sets; that is, any mixture of spectral colors is also a color.

File:CIE chromaticity diagram 2012 version.png of the Adobe RGB color space. The Planckian locus is shown with color temperatures labeled in kelvins. The outer curved boundary is the spectral locus, with wavelengths shown in nanometers. Note that the colors in this file are specified in Adobe RGB. Areas outside the triangle cannot be accurately rendered because they are out of the gamut of Adobe RGB, therefore they have been interpreted. Note that the colors depicted depend on the color space of the device you use to view the image (number of colors on your monitor, etc.), and may not be a strictly accurate representation of the color at a particular position.]]

In practice, it would be quite difficult to physiologically measure an individual's three cone responses to various physical color stimuli. Instead, a psychophysical approach is taken.{{cite journal | vauthors = Jacobs DE, Gallo O, Cooper EA, Pulli K, Levoy M | title = Simulating the Visual Experience of Very Bright and Very Dark Scenes. | journal = ACM Trans. Graph. | date = May 2015 | volume = 34 | issue = 3 | pages = 15 | url = https://graphics.stanford.edu/papers/gazehdr/ | doi = 10.1145/2714573 | s2cid = 14960893 | doi-access = free }} Three specific benchmark test lights are typically used; let us call them S, M, and L. To calibrate human perceptual space, scientists allowed human subjects to try to match any physical color by turning dials to create specific combinations of intensities (IS, IM, IL) for the S, M, and L lights, resp., until a match was found. This needed only to be done for physical colors that are spectral, since a linear combination of spectral colors will be matched by the same linear combination of their (IS, IM, IL) matches. Note that in practice, often at least one of S, M, L would have to be added with some intensity to the physical test color, and that combination matched by a linear combination of the remaining 2 lights. Across different individuals (without color blindness), the matchings turned out to be nearly identical.

By considering all the resulting combinations of intensities (IS, IM, IL) as a subset of 3-space, a model for human perceptual color space is formed. (Note that when one of S, M, L had to be added to the test color, its intensity was counted as negative.) Again, this turns out to be a (mathematical) cone, not a quadric, but rather all rays through the origin in 3-space passing through a certain convex set. Again, this cone has the property that moving directly away from the origin corresponds to increasing the intensity of the S, M, L lights proportionately. Again, a cross-section of this cone is a planar shape that is (by definition) the space of "chromaticities" (informally: distinct colors); one particular such cross-section, corresponding to constant X+Y+Z of the CIE 1931 color space, gives the CIE chromaticity diagram.

This system implies that for any hue or non-spectral color not on the boundary of the chromaticity diagram, there are infinitely many distinct physical spectra that are all perceived as that hue or color. So, in general, there is no such thing as the combination of spectral colors that we perceive as (say) a specific version of tan; instead, there are infinitely many possibilities that produce that exact color. The boundary colors that are pure spectral colors can be perceived only in response to light that is purely at the associated wavelength, while the boundary colors on the "line of purples" can each only be generated by a specific ratio of the pure violet and the pure red at the ends of the visible spectral colors.

The CIE chromaticity diagram is horseshoe-shaped, with its curved edge corresponding to all spectral colors (the spectral locus), and the remaining straight edge corresponding to the most saturated purples, mixtures of red and violet.

See also

References

{{Reflist}}

Further reading

{{refbegin}}

  • {{cite web | vauthors = Biggs T, McPhail S, Nassau K, Patankar H, Stenerson M, Maulana F, Douma M | veditors = Smith SE | url = http://www.webexhibits.org/causesofcolor/17.html | title = What colors do animals see? | work = Web Exhibits | publisher = Institute for Dynamic Educational Advancement (IDEA) }}
  • {{cite book | vauthors = Feynman RP | chapter = Color Vision | veditors = Gottlieb MA, Pfeiffer R|title=Feynman lectures on physics. Volume, Mainly mechanics, radiation, and heat |date=2015 |publisher=Basic Books |location=New York |isbn=978-0-465-04085-8 |edition=New millennium | chapter-url = https://feynmanlectures.caltech.edu/I_35.html | via = California Institute of Technology }}
  • {{cite journal | vauthors = Gouras P | url = http://webvision.med.utah.edu/book/part-vii-color-vision/color-vision/ | title = Color Vision | journal = Webvision | publisher = University of Utah School of Medicine | date = May 2009 | pmid = 21413395 }}
  • {{cite web | vauthors = McEvoy B |year=2008 | url = http://www.handprint.com/LS/CVS/color.html |access-date=2012-03-30 |title=Color vision }}
  • {{cite magazine | vauthors = Rogers A | url = https://www.wired.com/2015/02/science-one-agrees-color-dress/ | title = The Science of Why No One Agrees on the Color of This Dress | magazine = Wired | date = 26 February 2015 }}

{{refend}}

{{Color vision}}

{{Color topics}}

{{Mental processes}}

{{Authority control}}

Category:Image processing

Category:Visual perception

Category:Mathematics in medicine