Consonant voicing and devoicing
{{Short description|Phonetic sound change}}
{{Sound change}}
{{Use American English|date = February 2019}}
{{IPA notice}}
In phonology, voicing (or sonorization) is a sound change where a voiceless consonant becomes voiced due to the influence of its phonological environment; shift in the opposite direction is referred to as devoicing or surdization. Most commonly, the change is a result of sound assimilation with an adjacent sound of opposite voicing, but it can also occur word-finally or in contact with a specific vowel.
For example, the English suffix -s is pronounced {{IPA|[s]}} when it follows a voiceless phoneme (cats), and {{IPA|[z]}} when it follows a voiced phoneme (dogs).{{sfnp|Grijzenhout|2000|p=3}} This type of assimilation is called progressive, where the second consonant assimilates to the first; regressive assimilation goes in the opposite direction, as can be seen in have to {{IPA|[hæftə]}}.
==English==
English no longer has a productive process of voicing stem-final fricatives when forming noun-verb pairs or plural nouns, but there are still examples of voicing from earlier in the history of English:
- belief ({{IPA|[f]}}) – believe ({{IPA|[v]}})
- shelf ({{IPA|[f]}}) – shelve ({{IPA|[v]}})
- grief ({{IPA|[f]}}) – grieve ({{IPA|[v]}})
- life ({{IPA|[f]}}) – live ({{IPA|[v]}})
- proof ({{IPA|[f]}}) – prove ({{IPA|[v]}})
- strife ({{IPA|[f]}}) – strive ({{IPA|[v]}})
- thief ({{IPA|[f]}}) – thieve ({{IPA|[v]}})
- bath ({{IPA|[θ]}}) - bathe ({{IPA|[ð]}})
- breath ({{IPA|[θ]}}) - breathe ({{IPA|[ð]}})
- mouth ({{IPA|[θ]}}, {{abbr|n.|noun}}) – mouth ({{IPA|[ð]}}, {{abbr|vb.|verb}})
- sheath ({{IPA|[θ]}}) - sheathe ({{IPA|[ð]}})
- wreath ({{IPA|[θ]}}) - wreathe ({{IPA|[ð]}})
- advice ({{IPA|[s]}}) – advise ({{IPA|[z]}})
- house ({{IPA|[s]}}, {{abbr|n.|noun}}) – house ({{IPA|[z]}}, {{abbr|vb.|verb}})
- use ({{IPA|[s]}}, {{abbr|n.|noun}}) – use ({{IPA|[z]}}, {{abbr|vb.|verb}})
Synchronically, the assimilation at morpheme boundaries is still productive, such as in:{{sfnp|Grijzenhout|2000|p=9}}
- cat + s → cats
- dog + s → dogs ({{IPA|[ɡz]}})
- miss + ed → missed ({{IPA|[st]}})
- whizz + ed → whizzed ({{IPA|[zd]}})
The voicing alternation found in plural formation is losing ground in the modern language,{{Citation needed|date=December 2009}}. Of the alternations listed below many speakers retain only the {{IPA|[f-v]}} pattern, which is supported by the orthography. This voicing of {{IPA|/f/}} is a relic of Old English, at a time when the unvoiced consonants between voiced vowels were 'colored' by an allophonic voicing (lenition) rule {{IPA|/f/}} → {{IPA|[v]}}. As the language became more analytic and less inflectional, final vowels or syllables stopped being pronounced. For example, modern knives is a one syllable word instead of a two syllable word, with the vowel e not pronounced and no longer part of the word's structure. The voicing alternation between {{IPA|[f]}} and {{IPA|[v]}} occurs now as realizations of separate phonemes {{IPA|/f/}} and {{IPA|/v/}}. The alternation pattern is well maintained for the items listed immediately below, but its loss as a productive allophonic rule permits its abandonment for new usages of even well-established terms: while leaf~leaves in reference to 'outgrowth of plant stem' remains vigorous, the Toronto ice hockey team is uncontroversially named the Maple Leafs.
- knife – knives
- leaf – leaves
- wife – wives
- wolf – wolves
The following mutations are optional:{{Citation needed|date=December 2009}}
- bath ({{IPA|[θ]}}) - baths ({{IPA|[ð]}})
- mouth ({{IPA|[θ]}}) - mouths ({{IPA|[ð]}})
- oath ({{IPA|[θ]}}) - oaths ({{IPA|[ð]}})
- path ({{IPA|[θ]}}) - paths ({{IPA|[ð]}})
- youth ({{IPA|[θ]}}) - youths ({{IPA|[ð]}})
- house ({{IPA|[s]}}) – houses ({{IPA|[z]}})
Sonorants ({{IPA|/l r w j/}}) following aspirated fortis plosives (that is, {{IPA|/p t k/}} in the onsets of stressed syllables unless preceded by {{IPA|/s/}}) are devoiced such as in please, crack, twin, and pewter.{{sfnp|Roach|2004|p=240}}
Several varieties of English have a productive synchronic rule of /t/-voicing whereby intervocalic /t/ not followed by a stressed vowel is realized as voiced alveolar flap [ɾ], as in tutor, with the first /t/ pronounced as voiceless aspirated [tʰ] and the second as voiced [ɾ]. Voiced phoneme /d/ can also emerge as [ɾ], so that tutor and Tudor may be homophones, both with [ɾ] (the voiceless identity of word-internal /t/ in tutor is manifested in tutorial, where stress shift assures [tʰ]).
In other languages
= Voicing assimilation =
{{main|Assimilation (linguistics)}}
In many languages, including Polish and Russian, there is anticipatory assimilation of unvoiced obstruents immediately before voiced obstruents. For example, Russian {{lang|ru|просьба}} 'request' is pronounced {{IPA|/ˈprozʲbə/}} (instead of {{IPA|*/ˈprosʲbə/}}) and Polish {{lang|pl|prośba}} 'request' is pronounced {{IPA|/ˈprɔʑba/}} (instead of {{IPA|*/ˈprɔɕba/}}). The process can cross word boundaries as well: Russian {{lang|ru|дочь бы}} {{IPA|/ˈdod͡ʑ bɨ/}} 'daughter would'. The opposite type of anticipatory assimilation happens to voiced obstruents before unvoiced ones: {{lang|ru|обсыпать}} {{IPA|/ɐpˈs̪ɨpətʲ/}}.
In Italian, {{IPA|/s/}} before a voiced consonant is pronounced {{IPA|[z]}} within any phonological word: {{lang|it|sbaglio}} {{IPA|[ˈzbaʎʎo]}} 'mistake', {{lang|it|slitta}} {{IPA|[ˈzlitta]}} 'sled', {{lang|it|snello}} {{IPA|[ˈznɛllo]}} 'slender'. The rule applies across morpheme boundaries ({{lang|it|disdire}} {{IPA|[dizˈdiːre]}} 'cancel') and word boundaries ({{lang|it|lapis nero}} {{IPA|[ˌlaːpizˈneːro]}} 'black pencil'). This voicing is productive and so it applies also to borrowings, not only to native lexicon: {{lang|it|snob}} {{IPA|[znɔb]}}.
= Final devoicing =
{{main|Final-obstruent devoicing}}
Final devoicing is a systematic phonological process occurring in languages such as German, Dutch, Polish, Russian and Catalan.{{cite journal |date=2 June 2009 |editor1-last=Nasukawa |editor1-first=Kuniya |editor2-last=Backley |editor2-first=Phillip |title=Strength Relations in Phonology |journal=Studies in Generative Grammar |volume=103 |isbn=9783110218596}}{{Page needed|reason=This is a whole journal. Which paper verifies this fact?|date=November 2022}} Such languages have voiced obstruents in the syllable coda or at the end of a word become voiceless.
= Initial voicing =
Initial voicing is a process of historical sound change in which voiceless consonants become voiced at the beginning of a word. For example, modern German {{lang|de|sagen}} {{IPA|[ˈzaːɡn̩]}}, Yiddish {{lang|yi|זאָגן|rtl=yes}} {{IPA|[ˈzɔɡn̩]}}, and Dutch {{lang|nl|zeggen}} {{IPA|[ˈzɛɣə]}} (all "say") all begin with {{IPA|[z]}}, which derives from {{IPA|[s]}} in an earlier stage of Germanic, as is still attested in English say, Swedish {{lang|sv|säga}} {{IPA|[ˈsɛjːa]}}, and Icelandic {{lang|is|segja}} {{IPA|[ˈseiːja]}}. Some English dialects were affected as well, but it is rare in Modern English. One example is fox (with the original consonant) compared to vixen (with a voiced consonant).
Notes
{{reflist}}
References
- {{Citation
|last=Roach
|first=Peter
|year=2004
|title=British English: Received Pronunciation
|journal=Journal of the International Phonetic Association
|volume=34
|issue=2
|pages=239–245
|doi=10.1017/S0025100304001768
|doi-broken-date=5 April 2025
|doi-access=free
}}
- {{Citation
|last=Grijzenhout
|first=Janet
|year=2000
|title=Voicing and devoicing in English, German, and Dutch; evidence for domain-specific identity constraints
|url=http://user.phil-fak.uni-duesseldorf.de/~grijzenh/sfb116-voice.PDF
|access-date=2009-12-18
|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20110719084837/http://user.phil-fak.uni-duesseldorf.de/~grijzenh/sfb116-voice.PDF
|archive-date=2011-07-19
|url-status=dead
}}