Domain-specific learning
{{Short description|Neurological theory}}
Domain-specific learning theories of development hold that we have many independent, specialised knowledge structures (domains), rather than one cohesive knowledge structure. Thus, training in one domain may not impact another independent domain.Siegler, R. (2006). How Children Develop: Exploring Child Develop Student Media Tool Kit & Scientific American Reader to Accompany How Children Develop. New York: Worth Publishers. {{ISBN|0-7167-6113-0}}. Domain-general views instead suggest that children possess a "general developmental function" where skills are interrelated through a single cognitive system.Bjorklund, D.F. & Causey, K.B. (2017). Children's Thinking: Cognitive Development and Individual Differences. Sage Publications. pp. 154-155 {{ISBN|9781506334349}} Therefore, whereas domain-general theories would propose that acquisition of language and mathematical skill are developed by the same broad set of cognitive skills, domain-specific theories would propose that they are genetically, neurologically and computationally independent.
Domain specificity has been supported by a variety of theorists. An early supporter was Jerry Fodor, who argued that the mind functions partly, by innate, domain-specific mental modules.Fodor, J.A. (1983). The Modularity of Mind. Cambridge: MIT Press. pp.119-128 {{ISBN|9780262260701}} In Modularity of Mind, Fodor proposed the Hypothesis of Modest Modularity, stating that input systems such as perception and language are modular, whereas central systems such as belief fixation and practical reasoning are not.{{Cite web|last=Robbins|first=P.|date=2017|title=Modularity of Mind|url=https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/win2017/entries/modularity-mind/|url-status=live|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20190318071553/https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/win2017/entries/modularity-mind/ |archive-date=2019-03-18 |access-date=|website=Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy}} {{ISSN|1095-5054}} By contrast, evolutionary psychologists have supported the Massive Modularity Hypothesis, arguing that the mind is not just partially, but completely modular,Samuels, R. (1998). Evolutionary Psychology and the Massive Modularity Hypothesis. The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science, 49(4), 575-602. https://doi.org/10.1093/bjps/49.4.575 {{ISSN|0007-0882}} composed of domain-specific modules genetically shaped by selection pressures to carry out innate and complex functions.Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2013). Evolutionary Psychology: New Perspectives on Cognition and Motivation. Annual Review of Psychology, 64(1), 201-229. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.121208.131628 {{ISSN|1545-2085}}Cosmides, L., Barrett, H.C. & Tooby, J. (2010). Adaptive specializations, social exchange, and the evolution of human intelligence. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences - PNAS, 107(Supplement 2), 9007-9014. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0914623107 {{ISSN|1091-6490}} Core knowledge theorists such as Elizabeth Spelke hold that knowledge can be separated into a few, highly specialised, domain-specific bodies.Spelke, E.S., & Kinzler, K.D. (2007). Core knowledge. Developmental Science, 10(1), 89-96. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00569.x {{ISSN|1467-7687}}
Domain-specific learning mechanisms
= Language =
The Poverty of the Stimulus (PoS) argument proposed by Noam Chomsky takes a nativist view towards language acquisition suggesting that innate, domain-specific knowledge structures help us to navigate tough linguistic environments. This flows contrary to empiricist views that learning and knowledge derive from our sensory experiences.Laurence, S. & Margolis, E. (2001). The Poverty of the Stimulus Argument. The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science, 52(2), 217-276. https://doi.org/10.1093/bjps/52.2.217 {{ISSN|0007-0882}} The PoS argument maintains that there is a mismatch between the linguistic knowledge that we acquire, and how much information is available to us in the environment.Berwick, R.C., Pietroski, P., Yankama, B. Chomsky, N. (2011). Poverty of the Stimulus Revisited. Cognitive Science, 35(7), 1207-1242. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1551-6709.2011.01189.x {{ISSN|1551-6709}}
Chomsky believed that children cannot be empiricist learners of language, because many linguistic principles are neither simple nor natural to acquire. Therefore, a sufficient linguistic environment would be required to facilitate a full understanding of language. However, the data needed to grasp these linguistic principles is not always available due to different environmental conditions. Despite this, all normal children are still able to formulate an accurate representation of grammar which led Chomsky to theorise that children must have an innate, domain-specific capacity for language.
== Further support for nativism ==
1. Biological time-clock
Evidence shows that children go through similar stages of language development at similar times, leading to many linguists advocating for an innate and pre-determined linguistic schedule.Aitchison, J. (1989). The articulate mammal: An introduction to psycholinguistics (3rd ed.). Routledge. pp. 66-90 {{ISBN|0-415-16791-4}}
class="wikitable"
|+Table 1 - The rough milestones of language development !Language stage !Beginning age |
Crying
|Birth |
Cooing
|6 weeks |
Babbling
|6 months |
Intonation patterns
|8 months |
1-word utterances
|1 year |
2-word utterances
|18 months |
Word inflections
|2 years |
Questions, negatives
|2{{frac|1|4}} years |
Rare or complex constructions
|5 years |
Mature speech
|10 years |
2. Predictability of error
Children explore a diverse range of grammars in their environment as they develop. Under empiric learning, this would likely cause them to make all kinds of unpredictable linguistic errors. However, children make errors that exhibit regularity. When expressing verbs in past tense form, they often overgeneralise irregular forms such as came and saw into comed and seed to match "regular" forms such as loved and worked. The way children deal with environmental irregularity has therefore led to the proposition of a domain-specific language hypothesis space.
3. Specific Language Impairment (SLI)
A dissociation between intelligence and linguistic functioning has been shown in people with SLI.Pinker, S. (1994). The Language Instinct. Penguin Books. pp. 25-54. {{ISBN|0-14-017529-6}} Evidence has also indicated that people with Grammatical-SLI experience grammar-only deficits.Van der Lely, H.K.J. (2005). Domain-specific cognitive systems: Insight from Grammatical-SLI. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(2), 53-59. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.002 {{ISSN|1364-6613}} The case of SLI may therefore indicate an independent linguistic system.
== Criticisms ==
= Socialisation =
Socialisation is integral to a child's ability to acquire the necessary skills to function in a social environment. It has commonly been viewed as a product of domain-general learning, with the same organisational principles applying to child development, regardless of setting, task or developmental stage. Objections have therefore been raised on its unitary approach and lack of consideration for variation across contexts.Bugental, D.B. (2000). Acquisition of the Algorithms of Social Life. Psychological Bulletin, 126(2), 187-219. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.126.2.187 {{ISSN|0033-2909}}
Instead, researchers have proposed a socialisation process involving five domains, stating that different parent-child relationships serve different functions, rely on different ways to bring about behavioural change, and have different outcomes.Grusec, J.E. & Davidov, M. (2010). Integrating Different Perspectives on Socialization Theory and Research: A Domain-Specific Approach. Child Development, 81(3), 687-709. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01426.x {{ISSN|1467-8624}}
1. Protection
Primarily responsible for giving the child a sense of security through adopting a comforting parenting style. This results in children being able to better manage stress knowing that support will be available to them.
2. Reciprocity
Mutual compliance forms this relationship where parent and child fulfil each other's desires and treat each other as equals. The reciprocity domain nurtures a child's tendency to reciprocate, which can predict pro-social behaviour.
3. Control
The control domain involves parents who modify their child's behaviour through exerting the necessary amount of authority to achieve the socialisation agent's goals. Consequently, outcomes involving a child's ability to suppress conflicting desires to make the correct moral and principled judgements are typical.
4. Guided learning
Aims to effectively guide children's learning through strategies and feedback to help acquire the target knowledge and skills.
5. Group participation
Parents attempt to encourage shared identity for the child through promoting routines and rituals that reflect group norms. Successful outcomes involve children conforming to and adopting group values that build on their notions of social identity.
== Further research ==
Opposition to domain-specific learning
Although some arguments have supported domain-specific learning, there still remains debate about how we truly learn and develop.Sloutsky, V. M. (2010). Mechanisms of Cognitive Development: Domain‐General Learning or Domain‐Specific Constraints? Cognitive Science, 34(7), 1125-1130. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1551-6709.2010.01132.x {{ISSN|1551-6709}}
Support for domain-general learning include theories from Jean Piaget and Charles Spearman. Piaget argued that developments in domain-general cognitive architecture drives learning and conceptual change in his theory of cognitive development.Carey, S., Zaitchik, D., & Bascandziev, I. (2015). Theories of development: In dialog with Jean Piaget. Developmental Review, 38, 36-54. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2015.07.003 {{ISSN|1090-2406}} Similarly, Spearman proposed an underlying, domain-general g-factor (general intelligence) to explain one's performance on all types of mental tests.Gray, P.O. & Bjorklund, D. (2018). Psychology. New York: Macmillan Learning. pp.382-386 {{ISBN|978-1-319-15051-8}}
However, research has also introduced the possibility of a combination of domain-specific and domain-general learning mechanisms. In the mathematical field, it has been hypothesised that both mechanisms are at work and target arithmetic skills differently.Cowan, R. & Powell, D. (2014). The Contributions of Domain-General and Numerical Factors to Third-Grade Arithmetic Skills and Mathematical Learning Disability. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106(1), 214-229. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0034097 {{ISSN|0022-0663}} It has further been suggested that the magnitude of each mechanism in determining mathematical achievement varies across grades.Geary, D.C., Nicholas, A., Li, Y. & Sun, J. (2017). Developmental Change in the Influence of Domain-General Abilities and Domain-Specific Knowledge on Mathematics Achievement: An Eight-Year Longitudinal Study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 109(5), 680-693. https://dx.doi.org/10.1037%2Fedu0000159 {{ISSN|0022-0663}} Therefore, research is needed to better understand our learning across a wide range of fields.