Halo effect#As a business model

{{Short description|Tendency for positive impressions to contaminate other evaluations}}

{{About|the cognitive bias||The Halo Effect (disambiguation){{!}}The Halo Effect}}

{{Copy edit|date=March 2024}}

The halo effect (sometimes called the halo error) is the tendency for positive impressions of a person, company, country, brand, or product in one area to positively influence one's opinion or feelings.{{Cite web | url=https://en.oxforddictionaries.com/definition/halo_effect | archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20170802083537/https://en.oxforddictionaries.com/definition/halo_effect | url-status=dead | archive-date=August 2, 2017 |title = Halo Effect | Definition of Halo Effect by Lexico}}{{cite news

|newspaper=Advertising Age |author=Al Ries |date=17 Apr 2006

|title=Understanding Marketing Psychology and the Halo Effect

|url=http://adage.com/article/al-ries/understanding-marketing-psychology-halo-effect/108676

|publisher=Crain Publications |access-date=2017-07-31}} The halo effect is "the name given to the phenomenon whereby evaluators tend to be influenced by their previous judgments of performance or personality."{{Cite journal|last1=Bethel|first1=Ann|last2=Knapp|first2=T|date=2010|title=Halo Effect|journal=Dictionary of Nursing and Research|volume=4th|via=Credo Reference}} The halo effect is a cognitive bias which can prevent someone from forming an image of a person, a product or a brand based on the sum of all objective circumstances at hand.

The term was coined by Edward Thorndike. A simplified example of the halo effect is when a person, after noticing that an individual in a photograph is attractive, well groomed, and properly attired, then assumes, using a mental heuristic, that the person in the photograph is a good person based upon the rules of their own social concept.{{cite journal |last1=Nisbett |first1=Richard E. |last2=Wilson |first2=Timothy D. |title=The halo effect: Evidence for unconscious alteration of judgments. |journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology |date=1977 |volume=35 |issue=4 |pages=250–256 |doi=10.1037/0022-3514.35.4.250 |hdl=2027.42/92158 |s2cid=17867385 |hdl-access=free }}{{cite journal |last1=Gibson |first1=Jeremy L. |last2=Gore |first2=Jonathan S. |title=Is He a Hero or a Weirdo? How Norm Violations Influence the Halo Effect |journal=Gender Issues |date=December 2016 |volume=33 |issue=4 |pages=299–310 |doi=10.1007/s12147-016-9173-6 |s2cid=151849084 }} This constant error in judgment is reflective of the individual's preferences, prejudices, ideology, aspirations, and social perception.{{cite journal |last1=Lachman |first1=Sheldon J. |last2=Bass |first2=Alan R. |title=A Direct Study of Halo Effect |journal=The Journal of Psychology |date=November 1985 |volume=119 |issue=6 |pages=535–540 |doi=10.1080/00223980.1985.9915460 }}{{cite journal |last1=Wade |first1=T. Joel |last2=DiMaria |first2=Cristina |title=Weight Halo Effects: Individual Differences in Perceived Life Success as a Function of Women's Race and Weight |journal=Sex Roles |date=1 May 2003 |volume=48 |issue=9 |pages=461–465 |doi=10.1023/A:1023582629538 |s2cid=141143275 }}{{cite journal |last1=Greenwald |first1=Anthony G. |last2=Banaji |first2=Mahzarin R. |title=Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. |journal=Psychological Review |date=1995 |volume=102 |issue=1 |pages=4–27 |doi=10.1037/0033-295x.102.1.4 |pmid=7878162 |s2cid=8194189 |citeseerx=10.1.1.411.2919 }}{{cite journal |last1=Levy |first1=Leon H. |last2=Dugan |first2=Robert D. |title=A constant error approach to the study of dimensions of social perception. |journal=The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology |date=July 1960 |volume=61 |issue=1 |pages=21–24 |doi=10.1037/h0042208 |pmid=14416418 }}

Context and applications

=Psychology=

The halo effect is a perception distortion (or cognitive bias) that affects the way people interpret the information about someone with whom they have formed a positive gestalt.{{cite book|title=Elsevier's Dictionary of Psychological Theories|last1=Roeckelein|first1=Jon. E|date=2006|publisher=Elsevier Science & Technology|edition=1st}} An example of the halo effect is when a person finds out someone they have formed a positive gestalt with has cheated on their taxes. Because of the positive gestalt, the person may dismiss the significance of this behavior. They may even think that the person simply made a mistake. The person would justify the behavior and connect it with the other person's positive gestalt. The halo effect refers to the tendency to evaluate an individual positively on many traits because of a shared belief.{{cite book|title=Choices & Connections|last1=McCornack|first1=Steven|edition=2nd}}

It is a type of immediate judgment discrepancy, or cognitive bias, in which a person making an initial assessment of another person, place, or thing will assume ambiguous information based upon concrete information.{{Cite book|title=Thinking, fast and slow|author=Daniel Kahneman|publisher=Farrar, Straus, and Giroux|year=2013|isbn=978-0-374-53355-7|edition=1st|location=New York|pages=82–88}}{{Cite book|title=The Halo Effect and the Eight Other Business Delusions That Deceive Managers|author=Philip M. Rosenzweig|publisher=Free Press|year=2014|isbn=978-1-4767-8403-8|location=New York, NY}}{{rp|p. xi}} The halo effect is an evaluation by an individual and can affect the perception of a decision, action, idea, business, person, group, entity, or other whenever concrete data is generalized or influences ambiguous information.{{rp|11}}{{Cite journal|last1=Zhang|first1=Zhongheng|last2=Poucke|first2=Sven Van|date=2017-01-03|title=Citations for Randomized Controlled Trials in Sepsis Literature: The Halo Effect Caused by Journal Impact Factor|journal=PLOS ONE|volume=12|issue=1|pages=e0169398|bibcode=2017PLoSO..1269398Z|doi=10.1371/journal.pone.0169398|pmc=5207738|pmid=28046105|doi-access=free}}

The halo effect can also be explained as the behavior (usually unconscious) of using evaluations based on unrelated criteria, to make judgments about something or someone. The halo effect is sometimes used to refer specifically to when this behavior has a positive correlation, such as viewing someone who is attractive as likely to be successful and popular. When this judgment has a negative connotation, such as when someone unattractive is more readily blamed for a crime than someone attractive, it is sometimes referred to as the horn effect.{{Cite journal|last1=Sigall|first1=Harold|last2=Ostrove|first2=Nancy|date=1975-03-01|title=Beautiful but Dangerous: Effects of Offender Attractiveness and Nature of the Crime on Juridic Judgment|url=https://www.researchgate.net/publication/232451231|journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology|volume=31|issue=3|pages=410–414|doi=10.1037/h0076472}}

=Marketing=

The term halo effect is used in marketing to explain consumer bias toward certain products because of favorable experience with other products made by the same company.{{Cite news |url=https://www.investopedia.com/terms/h/halo-effect.asp |title=Halo Effect |date=27 October 2008 |access-date=2017-12-09}} It is used in the part of brand marketing called "line extensions". One common halo effect is when the perceived positive features of a particular item extend to a broader brand. A notable example is the manner in which the popularity of Apple's iPod generated enthusiasm for the corporation's other products.{{cite news |title=Apple shares surfs on big profits |url=http://news.bbc.co.uk/1/hi/business/4172211.stm |access-date=18 January 2012 |newspaper=BBC News |date= 13 January 2005}}{{cite news |newspaper=Advertising Age |url=https://adage.com/article/news/apple-puts-ipod-halo-test-shuffle-mini/101786/ |title=Apple puts iPod halo to test with Shuffle and Mini |date=17 January 2005}} Advertising often makes use of television shows, movies and those who star in them, to promote products via the halo effect.{{cite news |newspaper=Advertising Age |url=https://adage.com/article/media/advertisers-emmy-halo-effect-thing-past/2184026 |title=For advertisers, the Emmy halo effect is a thing of the past |author=Anthony Crupi |date=July 16, 2019}}{{cite news |newspaper=The New York Times |url=https://www.nytimes.com/2017/08/01/fashion/wonder-woman-leather-armor-whitaker-malem.html |title=The Wonder Woman Effect |author=Melanie Abrams |date=August 1, 2017}}

In the automotive industry, exotic, limited-production luxury models or low-volume sports cars made by a manufacturer's racing, motorsports, or in-house modification teams, are sometimes referred to as "halo cars" for the effect they are intended to produce on selling other vehicles within the make.{{cite magazine |magazine=Popular Mechanics |url=https://www.popularmechanics.com/cars/a8075/what-good-is-a-halo-car-anyway-12206624 |title=What good is a halo car anyway? |last=Frank |first=Michael |date=31 August 2012 |publisher=Hearst Communications, Inc. |access-date=12 December 2018}} To contrast this with the automotive terminology "flagship model", see here.

In the wine industry, certain wine features create a Halo Effect that can influence the customer’s opinion of a given wine. The inclusion of the category “organic” on the label of a wine can increase the consumer’s positive valuation of the wine. Organic wines are conceived of as being healthy, having a better taste, scent, and color, and result in a higher degree of overall satisfaction.{{cite journal |journal=Journal of Sensory Studies |url=https://doi.org/10.1111/joss.12243 |title=Organic label's halo effect on sensory and hedonic experience of wine: A pilot study |last1=Apaolaza |first1=Vanessa |last2=Hartmann |first2=Patrick |last3=Echebarria |first3=Carmen |last4=Barrutia |first4=Jose |date=30 January 2017 |publisher=Wiley Blackwell |access-date=17 February 2025}} Another example of the halo effect in the wine industry is the association of traditional corks with wine quality: corked bottles are systematically rated as of higher quality than bottles that use screw caps and plastic caps since the latter are viewed as signifiers of low-quality wines.{{cite journal |journal=International Journal of Hospitality Management |url=https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0278431918302512#preview-section-cited-by |title=What effect does wine bottle closure type have on perceptions of wine attributes? |last1=Reynolds |first1=Dennis |last2=Rahman |first2=Imran |last3=Bernard |first3=Shaniel |last4=Holbrook |first4=Amy |date=September 2018 |publisher=Emerald Publishing |access-date=17 February 2025}}

Advertising in one channel has been shown to have a halo effect on advertising in another channel.{{cite journal |last1=Rowson |first1=Paul |last2=Thompson |first2=Howard |last3=Berry |first3=Julian |title=Using a decision support optimisation software tool to maximise returns from an overall marketing budget: A case study from a B-to-C marketing company |journal=Journal of Database Marketing & Customer Strategy Management |date=1 June 2012 |volume=19 |issue=2 |pages=138–142 |doi=10.1057/dbm.2012.10 |s2cid=167892664 |doi-access=free }}{{cite journal |last1=Timothy Coombs |first1=W. |last2=Holladay |first2=Sherry J. |title=Unpacking the halo effect: reputation and crisis management |journal=Journal of Communication Management |date=April 2006 |volume=10 |issue=2 |pages=123–137 |doi=10.1108/13632540610664698 }}

A halo effect with regard to health, dubbed a "health halo", is used in food marketing to increase sales of a product; it can result in increased consumption of the product in the halo which may be unhealthy.{{cite journal |last1=Provencher |first1=Véronique |last2=Jacob |first2=Raphaëlle |title=Impact of Perceived Healthiness of Food on Food Choices and Intake |journal=Current Obesity Reports |date=March 2016 |volume=5 |issue=1 |pages=65–71 |doi=10.1007/s13679-016-0192-0 |pmid=26820622 |hdl=20.500.11794/13474 |s2cid=207474222 |hdl-access=free}}{{cite journal |last1=Polivy |first1=Janet |title=What's that you're eating? Social comparison and eating behavior |journal=Journal of Eating Disorders |date=December 2017 |volume=5 |issue=1 |pages=18 |doi=10.1186/s40337-017-0148-0 |pmid=28465828 |pmc=5408479 |doi-access=free }}

The term "halo effect" has also been applied to human rights organizations that have used their status to move away from their stated goals. Political scientist Gerald Steinberg has claimed that non-governmental organizations (NGOs) take advantage of the halo effect and are "given the status of impartial moral watchdogs" by governments and the news media.{{cite news |author=Nathan Jeffray |title=Interview: Gerald Steinberg |url = http://www.thejc.com/news/uk-news/33415/interview-gerald-steinberg |newspaper=The Jewish Chronicle |date=24 June 2010}}{{cite news |author=Naftali Balanson |title=The 'halo effect' shields NGOs from media scrutiny |url=http://www.jpost.com/Opinion/Op-EdContributors/Article.aspx?id=110648 |newspaper=The Jerusalem Post |date=8 October 2008}}

The Ronald McDonald House, a widely known NGO, openly celebrates the positive outcomes it receives from the halo effect. The web page for the Ronald McDonald House in Durham, North Carolina, states that 95% of survey participants were aware of Ronald McDonald House Charities. This awareness is attributed to the halo effect, as employees, customers, and stakeholders are more likely to be involved in a charity that they recognize and trust, with a name and logo that are familiar.{{cite web |url=http://www.ronaldhousedurham.org/page/corporate-donors |author=Nancy Jones |title=Corporate Donors |publisher=Ronald House Durham |access-date=26 November 2013 |archive-date=3 December 2013 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20131203013311/http://www.ronaldhousedurham.org/page/corporate-donors |url-status=dead }}

A brand's halo effect can protect its reputation in the event of a crisis. An event that is detrimental to a brand that is viewed favorably would not be as threatening or damaging to a brand that consumers view unfavorably.{{cite journal |last1 =Coombs |first1 =Timothy W |last2 = Holladay |first2 =Sherry J |journal=Journal of Communication Management |year=2006 |volume=10 |issue= 2 |pages = 123–37 |doi=10.1108/13632540610664698 |title=Unpacking the halo effect: reputation and crisis management}}{{cite journal |last1 =Klein |first1 =Jill |first2 = Niraj |last2 = Dawar |title= Evaluations in a Product-Harm Crisis |journal= International Journal of Research in Marketing |year= 2004 |volume=21 |issue=3 |pages=203–17 |doi=10.1016/j.ijresmar.2003.12.003}}

=Other uses=

Non-psychology/business use of the term "halo effect" describes the monetary value of the spillover effect{{efn|related to Net Present Value}} when an organization's marketing budget is subsequently reduced.{{efn|The loss of recency is compensated from the effective frequency of advertising expenditures of prior periods.}} This was first demonstrated to students via the 1966 version of a textbook and a software package named "The Marketing Game."{{efn|The textbook has been revised more than once, and the mainframe program from 1966 is now a PC program.}}

The halo effect can also be used in the case of institutions as one's favorable perceptions regarding an aspect of an organization could determine a positive view of its entire operations.{{Cite book|title=Organizational Behavior, Theory, and Design in Health Care

|author=Nancy Borkowski |year=2015

|publisher=Jones & Bartlett Publishers |isbn=978-1-284-05088-2 |location=Burlington, MA |pages=63}} For example, if a hospital is known for its excellent open heart and cardiac program, then the community would expect it to excel in other areas as well. This can also be demonstrated in the positive perceptions of financial institutions that gained favorable coverage in the media due to meteoric growth but eventually failed afterward.{{Cite book|title=The Halo Effect: . . . and the Eight Other Business Delusions That Deceive Managers

|author=Phil Rosenzweig |year=2014 |publisher=Free Press |isbn=978-0-7432-9125-5 |location=New York |pages=xv}}

The term "halo effect" is also used in metal detecting {{Cite web|url=https://metaldetectoruniverse.com/metal-detectors-on-wet-ground//|title = Do Metal Detectors Work Better on Wet Ground?}} to denote the enhanced ability of a metal item or coin to be detectable when it has been left undisturbed for some period of time in wet soil. The object can leach some metallic properties into the soil, making it more detectable. The area surrounding the object is called its "halo."

History

The halo effect was originally identified in 1907 by the American psychologist Frederick L. Wells (1884–1964).{{cite journal |last1=Guha |first1=Martin |title=Elsevier's Dictionary of Psychological Theories2006405Compiled by J.E. Roeckelein. Elsevier's Dictionary of Psychological Theories. Amsterdam: Elsevier 2006. xii+679 pp. £90; $143, {{text|ISBN}}: 0 444 51750 2 |journal=Reference Reviews |date=December 2006 |volume=20 |issue=8 |pages=10–11 |doi=10.1108/09504120610709402 }} However, it was only officially recognized in 1920 with empirical evidence provided by the psychologist Edward Thorndike (1874–1949). Edward Thorndike was the first to say the halo effect is a specific cognitive bias in which one aspect of the person, brand, product, or institution affects one's thoughts or judgment of the entity's other aspects or dimensions.{{Cite book|last=Longe|first=Jacqueline|title=The Gale Encyclopedia of Psychology|year=2016|pages=507–509}} Thorndike, an early behaviorist, was an important contributor to the study of the psychology of learning. He gave the phenomenon its name in his 1920 article "A Constant Error in Psychological Ratings".{{harvnb|Thorndike|1920}} In "Constant Error", Thorndike set out to replicate the study in hopes of pinning down the bias that he thought was present in these ratings. Subsequent researchers have studied it in relation to attractiveness and its bearing on the judicial and educational systems. Thorndike originally coined the term referring only to people; however, its use has been greatly expanded, especially in the area of brand marketing.

=Supporting evidence=

In Thorndike's words, "Ratings were apparently affected by a marked tendency to think of the person in general as rather good or rather inferior and to color the judgments of the qualities by this general feeling."{{cite encyclopedia |last=Edward |first=Craighead |date=2004 |encyclopedia=The Concise Corsini Encyclopedia of Psychology and Behavioral Science |title=Rater Errors }} In "A Constant Error in Psychological Ratings", Thorndike asked two commanding officers to evaluate their soldiers in terms of physical qualities (neatness, voice, physique, bearing, and energy), intellect, leadership skills, and personal qualities (including dependability, loyalty, responsibility, selflessness, and cooperation). In Thorndike's study, attractiveness plays an important role in how people tend to consider a person, such as whether a person is friendly or not based on their physical appearance. His goal was to see how the ratings of one characteristic affected other characteristics.

Thorndike's study showed how there was too great a correlation in the commanding officers' responses. In his review, he stated, "The correlations are too high and too even. For example, for the three raters next studied[,] the average correlation for physique with intelligence is .31; for physique with leadership, .39; and for physique with character, .28".{{Sfnp | Thorndike | 1920 | p = 27}} The ratings of one of the special qualities of an officer often started a trend in the rating results. The halo effect is not an indication of the existence of a correlation, but instead indicates that the correlation is too high. Thorndike used the halo effect to describe both a positive and negative halo.

In 2023, a large study of 2748 participants found that the same individuals received significantly higher ratings of intelligence, trustworthiness, sociability and happiness after having applied a beauty filter. It found a correlation of .30 for intelligence, .20 for trustworthiness, .39 for sociability and .39 for happiness. However, the study also found that beautified men received significantly higher scores to their perceived intelligence compared to women.{{Cite journal |last1=Gulati |first1=Aditya |last2=Martínez-Garcia |first2=Marina |last3=Fernández |first3=Daniel |last4=Lozano |first4=Miguel Angel |last5=Lepri |first5=Bruno |last6=Oliver |first6=Nuria |date=2024-11-27 |title=What is beautiful is still good: the attractiveness halo effect in the era of beauty filters |journal=Royal Society Open Science |volume=11 |issue=11 |pages=240882 |doi=10.1098/rsos.240882 |pmc=11597472 |pmid=39606589|arxiv=2407.11981 |bibcode=2024RSOS...1140882G }}

Cognitive bias

Cognitive bias is a pattern in perception, interpretation, or judgment that consistently leads to an individual misunderstanding something about themselves or their social environment, leading to poor decision-making or irrational behavior.{{cite book

|author=Jeni Mcray |title="Cognitive bias." Leadership Glossary: Essential Terms for the 21st Century

|year=2015 |publisher=Mission Bell Media |edition=1st}} The halo effect is classified as a cognitive bias because the halo effect is a perception error that distorts the way a person sees someone, and cognitive bias is a perception error that distorts the way that people see themselves.

The term "halo" is used in analogy with the religious concept: a glowing circle crowning the heads of saints in countless medieval and Renaissance paintings, bathing the saint's face in heavenly light. The observer may be subject to overestimating the worth of the observed by the presence of a quality that adds light on the whole like a halo. In other words, observers tend to bend their judgement according to one patent characteristic of the person (the "halo") or a few of his traits,{{Cite book|title=Cognitive Biases in Visualizations|last=Ellis|first=Geoffrey|date=2018|publisher=Springer|isbn=978-3-319-95830-9|location=Cham, Switzerland|pages=168}} generalizing toward a judgement of that person's character (e.g., in the literal hagiologic case, "entirely good and worthy").

The effect works in both positive and negative directions (and is hence sometimes called the horns and halo effect). If the observer likes one aspect of something, they will have a positive predisposition toward everything about it. If the observer dislikes one aspect of something, they will have a negative predisposition toward everything about it.{{Citation|title=Horns and halo effect|url=http://www.thefreedictionary.com/horns+and+halo+effect|publisher=The Free Dictionary|access-date=September 30, 2015}}

Role of attractiveness

{{see also|Physical attractiveness stereotype}}

A person's attractiveness has also been found to produce a halo effect. Attractiveness contributes to the halo effect because it can be influenced by several specific traits.{{Cite book|title=The Halo Effect|last=LeClaire|first=Anne|publisher=Lake Union Publishing|year=2017}} These perceptions of attractiveness may affect judgments tied to personality traits. Physical attributes contribute to perceptions of attractiveness (e.g., physique, hair, eye color). For example, someone who is perceived as attractive, due in part to physical traits, may be more likely to be perceived as kind or intelligent. The role of attractiveness in producing the halo effect has been illustrated through a number of studies. Recent research, for example, has revealed that attractiveness may affect perceptions tied to life success and personality.{{Cite journal|doi= 10.1023/A:1023582629538|title=Weight Halo Effects: Individual Differences in Perceived Life Success as a Function of Women's Race and Weight|year= 2003|last1= Wade | first1 = T Joel|last2=DiMaria|first2=Cristina |s2cid=141143275|journal=Sex Roles|volume=48|issue=9/10|pages=461–465}} In this study, attractiveness was correlated with weight, indicating that attractiveness itself may be influenced by various specific traits. Trustworthiness and friendliness were included in the personality variables. People perceived as being more attractive were more likely to be perceived as trustworthy and friendly. What this suggests is that perceptions of attractiveness may influence a variety of other traits, which supports the concept of the halo effect.

=On personality=

People's first impressions of others influence their later decision to either approach or avoid those individuals.{{Cite journal|last=Lasky|first=Jack|date=2020|title=Halo Effect|url=https://library.dctabudhabi.ae/eds/detail?db=ers&an=100259346|journal=Salem Press Encyclopedia of Health}} When people first encounter someone, the information present about that individual is limited; therefore, people will use the information available to assume other characteristics about that person; for instance, observable behaviors such as eye contact, leaning forward, smiling and positive hand gestures (ex. steepling hands) are linked to positive emotions, while avoiding eye contact, leaning back, avoiding touch, and defensive hand gestures (ex. hands in pockets) or no gestures at all are linked to feelings of detachment. Besides that, another popular example used when referring to the halo effect is the phenomenon called the attractiveness stereotype or when encountering individuals who are similar to others in some aspects, like personality or life history like the school they attended.{{Cite web|last=SMES|first=HR for|date=2021-01-19|title=How to avoid the Halo Effect|url=https://hr4smes.com/halo-effect-avoid-it/|access-date=2021-06-18|website=HR for Small and Medium businesses|language=en-US|archive-date=2021-06-24|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20210624200857if_/https://hr4smes.com/halo-effect-avoid-it/|url-status=dead}} People tend to assume that physically attractive individuals are more likely to be more healthy, successful, courteous, containing higher moral standards, and greater social competence than other people; on the other hand, the attractiveness stereotype can also carry a negative connotation as some people may think of attractive people as less honest and more conceited than others.

{{Harvtxt|Dion|Berscheid|Walster|1972}} conducted a study on the relationship between attractiveness and the halo effect. Sixty students, thirty males and thirty females from the University of Minnesota took part in the experiment. Each subject was given three different photos to examine: one of an attractive individual, one of an individual of average attractiveness, and one of an unattractive individual.{{Cite journal

|date=1990|title=Citations Classics - Commentary on 'What is beautiful is good' (1972)|first=Karen K. |last=Dion|url=http://garfield.library.upenn.edu/classics1990/A1990EH31100001.pdf|journal=Current Contents|publisher=ISI}} The participants judged the photos' subjects along 27 different personality traits (including altruism, conventionality, self-assertiveness, stability, emotionality, trustworthiness, extraversion, kindness, and sexual promiscuity). Participants were then asked to predict the overall happiness the photos' subjects would feel for the rest of their lives, including marital happiness (least likely to get divorced), parental happiness (most likely to be a good parent), social and professional happiness (most likely to experience life fulfillment), and overall happiness. Finally, participants were asked if the subjects would hold a job of high status, medium status, or low status. Results showed that most of the participants overwhelmingly believed more attractive subjects have more socially desirable personality traits than either averagely attractive or unattractive subjects, would lead happier lives in general, have happier marriages, and have more career success, including holding more secure, prestigious jobs. Participants, however, believed that attractive individuals would be worse parents than both averagely-attractive and unattractive individuals.

=Academics and intelligence=

A study by {{harvtxt|Landy|Sigall|1974}} demonstrated the Halo Effect, looking at male judgments of female intelligence and competence on academic tasks. Sixty male undergraduate students rated the quality of essays which included both well- and poorly-written samples. One third were presented with a photo of an attractive female as author, another third with that of an unattractive female as author, and the last third were shown neither. On average, most of the participants gave significantly better writing evaluations for the more attractive author. On a scale of 1 to 9, the well-written essay by the attractive author received an average of 6.7 while the unattractive author received a 5.9 (with a 6.6 as a control). The gap was larger on the poor essay: the attractive author received an average of 5.2, the control a 4.7, and the unattractive author a 2.7, suggesting male readers are generally more willing to give physically attractive females the benefit of the doubt when performance is below standard than those not considered attractive.

Research conducted by {{harvtxt|Moore|Filippou|Perrett|2011}} sought residual cues to intelligence in female and male faces while attempting to control for the attractiveness halo effect. Over 300 photographs of Caucasian British college students were rated for perceived intelligence. The photographs that were scored lowest in perceived intelligence were used to create a low-intelligence composite face and those photographs that were scored highest in perceived intelligence were used to create a high-intelligence composite face. Both female and male faces of high- and low-perceived intelligence were created, resulting in four groups of composite faces. Participants for the study were recruited online; 164 female and 92 male heterosexual residents of the UK rated each of the composite faces for intelligence and attractiveness. Of the female composites, attractiveness seemed to be controlled as both the high- and low-perceived intelligence groups were rated as equally attractive. However, of the male face composites, the high-perceived intelligence group was rated as significantly more attractive than the low-perceived intelligence group, suggesting that either the authors could not adequately control for the attractiveness halo effect for the male composite photographs or that intelligence is an integral factor of attractiveness in high-intelligence male faces. The second part of the study found that the composites in the high-perceived intelligence group were rated highest in the factors of friendly and funny as markers of intelligence in both the female and male groups. While intelligence does not seem to be a factor that contributes to attractiveness in women, with regards to men, attractive faces are perceived to be more intelligent, friendly, and funny by women and men.

=Political effects=

Officeholders who create what The New York Times called "a living legacy" benefit from a halo effect when their overall accomplishments are subsequently evaluated.{{cite news |newspaper=The New York Times

|url=https://www.nytimes.com/1982/01/30/opinion/halo-effect.html

|title=Halo Effect |date=January 30, 1982}}{{efn|The Times was referring to F. D. Roosevelt and Ronald Reagan}}

Researchers have shown that perceived physical and vocal attractiveness (or their opposite) lead to bias in judgment.{{cite journal |last1=Surawski |first1=Melissa K. |last2=Ossoff |first2=Elizabeth P. |title=The effects of physical and vocal attractiveness on impression formation of politicians |journal=Current Psychology |date=March 2006 |volume=25 |issue=1 |pages=15–27 |doi=10.1007/s12144-006-1013-5 |s2cid=144978466 }}{{cite journal |last1=Herrmann |first1=Michael |last2=Shikano |first2=Susumu |title=Attractiveness and Facial Competence Bias Face-Based Inferences of Candidate Ideology |journal=Political Psychology |date=2016 |volume=37 |issue=3 |pages=401–417 |doi=10.1111/pops.12256 }} A 2010 study{{cite journal |last1=Verhulst |first1=Brad |last2=Lodge |first2=Milton |last3=Lavine |first3=Howard |title=The Attractiveness Halo: Why Some Candidates are Perceived More Favorably than Others |journal=Journal of Nonverbal Behavior |date=June 2010 |volume=34 |issue=2 |pages=111–117 |doi=10.1007/s10919-009-0084-z |s2cid=37559811 }} found that attractiveness and familiarity are strong predictors of decisions regarding who is put in a position of leadership. Judgments made following one-second exposures to side-by-side photos of two US congressional candidates were reasonably predictive of election outcomes. Similar studies {{Harv | Palmer | Peterson | 2012}} found that even when taking factual knowledge into account, candidates who were rated as more attractive were still perceived as more knowledgeable. Thus, beauty evaluations also emerge as major predictors of electoral success.{{cite journal |last1=Poutvaara |first1=Panu |last2=Jordahl |first2=Henrik |last3=Berggren |first3=Niclas |title=Faces of politicians: Babyfacedness predicts inferred competence but not electoral success |journal=Journal of Experimental Social Psychology |date=September 2009 |volume=45 |issue=5 |pages=1132–1135 |doi=10.1016/j.jesp.2009.06.007 |s2cid=3228508 |url=http://www.ssoar.info/ssoar/handle/document/30989 |hdl=10419/81455 |hdl-access=free }}{{cite journal |last1=Berggren |first1=Niclas |last2=Jordahl |first2=Henrik |last3=Poutvaara |first3=Panu |title=The right look: Conservative politicians look better and voters reward it |journal=Journal of Public Economics |date=February 2017 |volume=146 |pages=79–86 |doi=10.1016/j.jpubeco.2016.12.008 |doi-access=free |hdl=10419/81356 |hdl-access=free }}

=The judicial context=

Study results showing the influence of the halo effect in the judicial context exist:

  • {{Harvtxt | Efran | 1974}} found subjects were more lenient when sentencing attractive individuals than unattractive ones, even though exactly the same crime was committed. The researchers attributed the result to a societal perception that people with a high level of attractiveness are seen as more likely to have successful futures due to corresponding socially desirable traits.
  • {{Harvtxt | Monahan | 1941}} studied social workers who were accustomed to interacting with a diverse range of people and found that the majority experienced difficulty when asked to consider that a beautiful person was guilty of a crime.
  • A study presented two hypothetical crimes: a burglary and a swindle. The burglary involved a woman illegally obtaining a key and stealing $2,200 (equivalent to ${{inflation|US|2200|1975|r=-3|fmt=c}} today); the swindle involved a woman manipulating a man to invest $2,200 in a nonexistent corporation. The results showed that when the offense was not related to attractiveness (as in the burglary) the unattractive defendant was punished more severely than the attractive one. However, when the offense was related to attractiveness (the swindle), the attractive defendant was punished more severely than the unattractive one. The study imputes that the usual leniency given to the attractive woman (as a result of the halo effect) was negated or reversed when the nature of the crime involved her looks.

=Gender differences=

{{harvtxt|Kaplan|1978}} found that some women were influenced by the halo effect on attractiveness only when presented with members of the opposite sex. {{harvtxt|Dermer|Thiel|1975}} continued this line of research, going on to demonstrate that jealousy of an attractive individual has a slight effect in evaluation of that person. These works showed these halo effect more prevalent among females than males. Later research by {{harvtxt|Moore|Filippou|Perrett|2011}} was able to control for attractiveness in composite photographs of females who were perceived to be of high or low intelligence, while showing that the attractiveness halo effect was seen in high intelligent male composite faces by heterosexual residents of the UK. Either the halo effect is negated by feelings of jealousy in women{{cite journal |last1=Dermer |first1=Marshall |last2=Thiel |first2=Darrel L. |title=When beauty may fail |journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology |date=1975 |volume=31 |issue=6 |pages=1168–1176 |doi=10.1037/h0077085 |citeseerx=10.1.1.552.3491 }} or the halo effect is lessened when women are looking at same sex individuals{{cite journal |last1=Kaplan |first1=Robert M. |title=Is beauty talent? Sex interaction in the attractiveness halo effect |journal=Sex Roles |date=1 April 1978 |volume=4 |issue=2 |pages=195–204 |doi=10.1007/BF00287500 |s2cid=143581604 }} or the attractiveness halo effect can be controlled for in women.{{cite journal |last1=Moore |first1=F. R. |last2=Filippou |first2=D. |last3=Perrett |first3=D. I. |title=Intelligence and attractiveness in the face: Beyond the attractiveness halo effect |journal=Journal of Evolutionary Psychology |date=September 2011 |volume=9 |issue=3 |pages=205–217 |doi=10.1556/jep.9.2011.3.2 |doi-access=free }}

=Possible causes=

Rating error effect, mistakes made by raters when they use a rating scale, reflect the task competence of the rater, as well as the rater's sex, social position, race, religion, and age. Researchers showed that halo effect is one component of this error. Fisicaro and Lance introduced three explanatory models.{{cite journal |last1=Feeley |first1=Thomas Hugh |title=Comment on Halo Effects in Rating and Evaluation Research |journal=Human Communication Research |date=October 2002 |volume=28 |issue=4 |pages=578–586 |doi=10.1111/j.1468-2958.2002.tb00825.x }} The first model named the general impression model states that global evaluation affects the rating of individual characteristics.{{cite journal |last1=Gräf |first1=Michael |last2=Unkelbach |first2=Christian |title=Halo effects from agency behaviors and communion behaviors depend on social context: Why technicians benefit more from showing tidiness than nurses do: Halo effects depend on social context |journal=European Journal of Social Psychology |date=August 2018 |volume=48 |issue=5 |pages=701–717 |doi=10.1002/ejsp.2353 }} The salient dimension model states that how people perceive an individual characteristic affects their evaluation of other characteristics. The inadequate discrimination model refers to the rater's failure to identify different behaviors of the person being evaluated.

The reverse halo effect

The reverse halo effect occurs when positive evaluations of an individual cause negative consequences. Rater errors pose special problems for the issues of "reliability and validity". Furthermore, ratings that differ in time may accurately reflect a change in behavior even though this difference would demonstrate an artificial lack of reliability. A follow-up study with both men and women participants supported this, as well as showing that attractive women were expected to be conceited and have a higher socioeconomic status. Eagly et al. (1991) also commented on this phenomenon, showing that more attractive individuals of both sexes were expected to be higher in vanity and possibly egotistic.{{cite journal |last1=Eagly |first1=Alice H. |last2=Ashmore |first2=Richard D. |last3=Makhijani |first3=Mona G. |last4=Longo |first4=Laura C. |title=What is beautiful is good, but . . .: A meta-analytic review of research on the physical attractiveness stereotype. |journal=Psychological Bulletin |date=1991 |volume=110 |issue=1 |pages=109–128 |doi=10.1037/0033-2909.110.1.109 }} Applied instances of the reverse halo effect include negative evaluations of criminals who use their attractiveness to their advantage and rating a philosophical essay lower when written by a young female than an old male.{{cite journal |last1=Forgas |first1=Joseph P. |title=She just doesn't look like a philosopher…? Affective influences on the halo effect in impression formation: Mood and halo effects |journal=European Journal of Social Psychology |date=December 2011 |volume=41 |issue=7 |pages=812–817 |doi=10.1002/ejsp.842 }}

The horn effect

{{main article|Horn effect}}

A negative form of the halo effect, called the horn effect, the devil effect, or the reverse halo effect, allows a disliked trait or aspect of a person or product to negatively influence overall perception. Psychologists call it a "bias blind spot:"{{cite web

|title=Halo and horns effects in rating errors

|url=http://www.rightattitudes.com/2010/04/30/rating-errors-halo-effect-horns-effect |year= 2010}} "Individuals believe (that negative) traits are inter-connected."{{Cite web | url=https://www.joshuakennon.com/mental-model-horns-effect-and-halo-effect |title = Mental Model: Horns Effect and Halo Effect| date=12 November 2011 }} due to a negative first impression.{{cite book |last1=Kurian |first1=George Thomas |title=The AMA Dictionary of Business and Management |date=2013 |publisher=Publishing Division o f the American Management Association |edition=1st}}{{cite journal

|author1=Richard E Nisbett |author2=Timothy D Wilson |year=1977

|title=The halo effect: Evidence for unconscious alteration of judgments

|journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology |volume=35 |issue=4 |pages=250–56

|doi=10.1037/0022-3514.35.4.250| hdl = 2027.42/92158 |s2cid=17867385 |hdl-access=free }} The Guardian wrote of the devil effect in relation to Hugo Chavez: "Some leaders can become so demonized that it's impossible to assess their achievements and failures in a balanced way."{{cite news | last =Glennie |first=Jonathan|title=Hugo Chávez's reverse-halo effect

|url= https://www.theguardian.com/global-development/poverty-matters/2011/may/03/hugo-chavez-reverso-halo-effect

|newspaper=The Guardian |date=3 May 2011}} For those seen in a negative light, anything they do that is negative is exemplified, while the positive things they do are not seen, or are doubted.

Education

{{harvtxt|Abikoff|Courtney | Pelham | Koplewicz |1993}} found the halo effect is also present in the classroom. In this study, both regular and special education elementary school teachers watched videotapes of what they believed to be children in regular 4th-grade classrooms. In reality, the children were actors, depicting behaviors present in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), oppositional defiant disorder (ODD), or standard behavior. The teachers were asked to rate the frequency of hyperactive behaviors observed in the children. Teachers rated hyperactive behaviors accurately for children with ADHD; however, the ratings of hyperactivity were much higher for the children with ODD-like behaviors, showing a horn effect for children who appeared to have ODD.

{{Harvtxt | Foster | Ysseldyke | 1976}} also found the halo effect present in teachers' evaluations of children. Regular and special education elementary school teachers watched videos of a normal child whom they were told was either "emotionally disturbed", possessing a learning disorder, "mentally retarded", or "normal". The teachers then completed referral forms based on the child's behavior. The results showed that teachers held negative expectancies toward emotionally disturbed children, maintaining these expectancies even when presented with normal behavior. In addition, the "mentally retarded" label showed a greater degree of negative bias than the "emotionally disturbed" or "learning disabled" label.

=Observations=

"In the classroom, teachers are subject to the halo effect rating error when evaluating their students. For example, a teacher who sees a well-behaved student might tend to assume this student is also bright, diligent, and engaged before that teacher has objectively evaluated the student's capacity in these areas. When these types of halo effects occur, they can affect students' approval ratings in certain areas of functioning and can even affect students' grades."{{Cite book |url=https://sk.sagepub.com/ency/edvol/educationalpsychology/toc |access-date=2025-02-01 |website=sso.sagepub.com |date=2008 |doi=10.4135/9781412963848 |last1=Salkind |first1=Neil |title=Encyclopedia of Educational Psychology |isbn=978-1-4129-1688-2 }}

"In the work setting, the halo effect is most likely to show up in a supervisor's appraisal of a subordinate's job performance. In fact, the halo effect is probably the most common bias in performance appraisal. Think about what happens when a supervisor evaluates the performance of a subordinate. The supervisor may give prominence to a single characteristic of the employee, such as enthusiasm, and allow the entire evaluation to be colored by how he or she judges the employee on that one characteristic. Even though the employee may lack the requisite knowledge or ability to perform the job successfully, if the employee's work shows enthusiasm, the supervisor may very well give him or her a higher performance rating than is justified by knowledge or ability."{{Cite book |last1=Schneider |first1=Frank W. |url=https://books.google.com/books?id=TMIVujqHVwQC |title=Applied Social Psychology: Understanding and Addressing Social and Practical Problems |last2=Gruman |first2=Jamie A. |last3=Coutts |first3=Larry M. |date=2005 |publisher=SAGE |isbn=978-1-4129-1539-7 |language=en}}

Further research findings

{{harvtxt|Murphy|Jako|Anhalt|1993}} argue: "Since 1980, there have been a large number of studies dealing directly or indirectly with halo error in rating. Taken together, these studies suggest that all seven of the characteristics that have defined halo error for much of its history are problematic and that the assumptions that underlie some of them are demonstrably wrong." Their work claims that the assumption that the halo effect is always detrimental is incorrect, with some halo effects resulting in an increase in the accuracy of the rating. Additionally, they discuss the idea of "true halo"—the actual correlation between, for example, attractiveness and performance as an instructor—and "illusory halo," which refers to cognitive distortions, errors in observation and judgement, and the rating tendencies of the individual rater. They claim that any true differentiation between true and illusory halos is impossible in a real-world setting, because the different ratings are strongly influenced by the specific behaviors of the person observed by the raters.

A study by {{harvtxt|Forgas|2011}} states that one's mood can affect the degree of the halo effect's influence. When someone is in a favorable mood, the halo effect is more likely to be influential—this was demonstrated by study participants choosing between pictures of an elderly man with a beard and a young woman, and deciding which subject possessed more philosophical attributes. Additionally, when asked to list the happy, neutral, or negative times in their life, the halo effect was more evident in the perceptions of the participants who chose to write about happy prior experiences. Forgas's study suggests that when one is gauging the extent of the halo effect in a situation, one must consider the emotional state of the person making the judgment.

A 2013 report on "the link between disease and leader preferences" claimed that "congressional districts with a higher incidence of disease" were more likely to show a halo effect "on electoral outcomes."{{cite news |newspaper=The New York Times

|url=https://www.nytimes.com/2013/11/03/opinion/sunday/health-beauty-and-the-ballot.html

|title=Why Attractive Candidates Win

|author1=Andrew Edward White |author2=Douglas T. Kenrick |date=November 1, 2013}}

See also

Notes

{{notelist}}

References

{{reflist |32em}}

Bibliography

{{refbegin}}

  • {{cite journal |last1=Abikoff |first1=Howard |last2=Courtney |first2=Mary |last3=Pelham |first3=William E. |last4=Koplewicz |first4=Harold S. |title=Teachers' ratings of disruptive behaviors: The influence of halo effects |journal=Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology |date=October 1993 |volume=21 |issue=5 |pages=519–533 |doi=10.1007/BF00916317 |pmid=8294651 |s2cid=46270488 }}
  • {{cite journal |last1=Dion |first1=Karen |last2=Berscheid |first2=Ellen |last3=Walster |first3=Elaine |title=What is beautiful is good |journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology |date=1972 |volume=24 |issue=3 |pages=285–290 |doi=10.1037/h0033731 |pmid=4655540 |citeseerx=10.1.1.521.9955 |s2cid=10152052 }}
  • {{cite journal |last1=Efran |first1=Michael G. |title=The effect of physical appearance on the judgment of guilt, interpersonal attraction, and severity of recommended punishment in a simulated jury task |journal=Journal of Research in Personality |date=June 1974 |volume=8 |issue=1 |pages=45–54 |doi=10.1016/0092-6566(74)90044-0 }}
  • {{cite journal |last1=Foster |first1=Glen |last2=Ysseldyke |first2=James |title=Expectancy and halo effects as a result of artificially induced teacher bias |journal=Contemporary Educational Psychology |date=January 1976 |volume=1 |issue=1 |pages=37–45 |doi=10.1016/0361-476X(76)90005-9 }}
  • {{cite journal |last1=Kanazawa |first1=Satoshi |last2=Kobar |first2=Jody L. |title=Why beautiful people are more intelligent |journal=Intelligence |date=May 2004 |volume=32 |issue=3 |pages=227–243 |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2004.03.003 |citeseerx=10.1.1.106.8858 }}
  • {{cite journal |last1=Landy |first1=David |last2=Sigall |first2=Harold |title=Beauty is talent: Task evaluation as a function of the performer's physical attractiveness. |journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology |date=1974 |volume=29 |issue=3 |pages=299–304 |doi=10.1037/h0036018 }}
  • {{cite book |last=Monahan |first=F. |title=Women in Crime |year=1941 |publisher=Washburn |location=New York |url=https://books.google.com/books?id=Wg4iAAAAMAAJ }}
  • {{cite journal |last1=Murphy |first1=Kevin R. |last2=Jako |first2=Robert A. |last3=Anhalt |first3=Rebecca L. |title=Nature and consequences of halo error: A critical analysis. |journal=Journal of Applied Psychology |date=April 1993 |volume=78 |issue=2 |pages=218–225 |doi=10.1037/0021-9010.78.2.218 |s2cid=6394309 }}
  • {{cite conference |last1=Palmer |first1=Carl L. |last2=Peterson |first2=Rolfe D. |title=Beauty and the Pollster: Interviewer Bias in Subjective Evaluations of Respondent Characteristics |conference=Annual Meeting of the Midwest Political Science Association |location=Chicago IL |year=2012 |url=http://cas.illinoisstate.edu/clpalme/research/documents/Beauty_and_the_Pollster_revision.pdf |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20140309023825/http://cas.illinoisstate.edu/clpalme/research/documents/Beauty_and_the_Pollster_revision.pdf |archive-date=2014-03-09 |url-status=dead }}
  • {{cite journal |last1=Thorndike |first1=E.L. |title=A constant error in psychological ratings. |journal=Journal of Applied Psychology |date=1920 |volume=4 |issue=1 |pages=25–29 |doi=10.1037/h0071663 |url=https://zenodo.org/record/1429134 }}

{{refend}}

Further reading

  • {{cite journal |last1=Palmer |first1=Carl L. |last2=Peterson |first2=Rolfe D. |title=Halo Effects and the Attractiveness Premium in Perceptions of Political Expertise |journal=American Politics Research |date=March 2016 |volume=44 |issue=2 |pages=353–382 |doi=10.1177/1532673x15600517 |s2cid=147048780 }}
  • {{cite book |last=Chandra |first=Ramesh |title=Social development in India |year=2004 |publisher=Isha |location = Delhi, IN |isbn=978-81-8205-024-2}}
  • {{cite web |last=Dean |first=Jeremy |title=The Halo Effect: When Your Own Mind is a Mystery |url = http://www.spring.org.uk/2007/10/halo-effect-when-your-own-mind-is.php |publisher=PsyBlog |year= 2007}}
  • {{cite news |last=Steinberg |first=Gerald M |title=Human Rights NGOs Need a Monitor |url = http://forward.com/articles/122209/human-rights-ngos-need-a-monitor | newspaper=The Jewish Daily Forward |date= 30 December 2009}}
  • {{cite book|last=Sutherland|first=Stuart|title=Irrationality|year=2007|publisher=Pinter & Martin|location = London |isbn=978-1-905177-07-3|edition= reprint}}

{{Biases}}

{{authority control}}

Category:Cognitive biases

Category:Educational psychology

Category:1920 introductions

Category:Error