agreeableness

{{Short description|Personality trait}}

File:William-Adolphe Bouguereau - Agreeable Burden 1895.jpg, 1895)]]

Agreeableness is the personality trait of being kind, sympathetic, cooperative, warm, honest, straightforward, and considerate.[https://www.psychologytoday.com/us/basics/agreeableness "Agreeableness" at Psychology Today] Retrieved on February 21, 2024{{cite journal|last=Thompson|first=E.R.|title=Development and Validation of an International English Big-Five Mini-Markers|journal=Personality and Individual Differences|date=October 2008|volume=45|issue=6|pages=542–548|doi=10.1016/j.paid.2008.06.013}} In personality psychology, agreeableness is one of the five major dimensions of personality structure, reflecting individual differences in cooperation.{{cite book|last1=Graziano|first1=W.G.|last2=Eisenberg|first2=N.|year=1997|chapter=Agreeableness; A dimension of personality|editor-first1=R.|editor-last1=Hogan|editor-first2=S.|editor-last2=Briggs|editor-first3=J.|editor-last3=Johnson|title=Handbook of Personality Psychology|location=San Diego, Calif.|publisher=Academic Press}} People who score high on measures of agreeableness are empathetic and self-sacrificing, while those with low agreeableness are prone to selfishness, insincerity, and zero-sum thinking.{{multiref2

|1={{cite journal |last1=Bamford |first1=Joshua Michael Silberstein |last2=Davidson |first2=Jane Whitfield |title=Trait Empathy associated with Agreeableness and rhythmic entrainment in a spontaneous movement to music task: Preliminary exploratory investigations |journal=Musicae Scientiae |date=28 March 2017 |volume=23 |issue=1 |pages=5–24 |doi=10.1177/1029864917701536|s2cid=151504600 |url=http://urn.fi/URN:NBN:fi:jyu-202309285307 }}

|2={{cite journal |last1=Song |first1=Yang |title=Associations between empathy and big five personality traits among Chinese undergraduate medical students |journal=PLOS ONE |volume=12 |issue=2 |pages=e0171665 |date=10 February 2017 |doi=10.1371/journal.pone.0171665 |pmid=28187194 |pmc=5302826 |bibcode=2017PLoSO..1271665S |doi-access=free}}

}} Those who score low on agreeableness may show dark triad tendencies, such as narcissistic, antisocial, and manipulative behavior.{{cite journal |last1=Kaufman |first1=Scott Barry |last2=Yaden |first2=David Bryce |last3=Hyde |first3=Elizabeth |last4=Tsukayama |first4=Eli |title=The Light vs. Dark Triad of Personality: Contrasting Two Very Different Profiles of Human Nature |journal=Frontiers in Psychology |date=12 March 2019 |volume=10 |pages=467 |doi=10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00467|pmid=30914993 |pmc=6423069 |doi-access=free }}

Agreeableness is a superordinate trait, meaning it is a grouping of personality sub-traits that cluster together statistically. Some lower-level traits, or facets, that are commonly grouped under agreeableness include trust, straightforwardness, altruism, helpfulness, modesty, and tender-mindedness.{{cite book|title=Culture and Psychology: 5th Edition|publisher=Wadsworth-Cengage Learning|author=Matsumoto, D.|author2=Juang, L.|year=2012|location=Belmont, California|pages=271|isbn=978-1-111-34493-1}}

History

= Early trait research =

As is the case with all Big Five personality traits, the roots of the modern concept of agreeableness can be traced to a 1936 study by Gordon Allport and Henry S. Odbert.{{cite journal

| title = Trait names: A psycholexical study.

| author = Allport, G. W. |author2=Odbert, H. S.

| journal = Psychological Monographs

| year = 1936

| volume = 47

| issue = 1

| doi = 10.1037/h0093360

| pages=i–171

}} Seven years after that study, Raymond Cattell published a cluster analysis of the thousands of personality-related words identified by Allport and Odbert.{{cite journal

| title = The description of personality: Basic traits resolved into clusters.

| author = Cattell, R. B.

| journal = Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology

| date = October 1943

| volume = 38

| issue = 4

| pages = 476–506

| doi = 10.1037/h0054116

}} The clusters identified in this study served as a foundation for Cattell's further attempts to identify fundamental, universal, human personality factors.{{cite book

| title = The Description and Measurement of Personality

| publisher = World Book

| author = Cattell, R. B.

| location = New York

}} Cattell eventually determined 16 personality factors (16PF) by means of factor analysis. Further factor analyses revealed five higher-order, or "global", factors that encompass these 16.{{cite web

|url=http://www.ipat.com/about/16pf/Pages/default.aspx

|title=The 16PF® Questionnaire

|publisher=IPAT

|access-date=February 16, 2012

|url-status=dead

|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20120229034356/http://www.ipat.com/about/16pf/Pages/default.aspx

|archive-date=February 29, 2012

}} Although labelled "independence" by Cattell, one of the global factors identified by the 16PF Questionnaire was an early precursor to the modern concept of agreeableness.{{cite book

| title = Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research

| publisher = Guilford Press

| first1 = Lawrence A.

| last1 = Pervin PhD

| first2 = Oliver P.

| last2 = John PhD

| year = 1999

| location = New York

| pages = 102–138

| isbn = 978-1-57230-695-0

| edition = 2nd

}}

= Big Five =

Agreeableness in the five factor model of personality is most commonly measured by self-report, although peer-reports and third-party observation can also be used. Self-report measures are either lexical or based on statements.{{cite journal

| last = Goldberg

| first = L.R.

| author2 = Johnson, JA

| author3 = Eber, HW

| title = The international personality item pool and the future of public-domain personality measures

| journal = Journal of Research in Personality

| year = 2006

| volume = 40

| issue = 1

| pages = 84–96

| doi = 10.1016/j.jrp.2005.08.007

| s2cid = 13274640

| display-authors = etal

}} Which measure is used depends on {{vague|text=an assessment of psychometric properties|date=July 2023}} and the time and space constraints of the research being undertaken.

Lexical measures use individual adjectives that reflect agreeableness or disagreeableness traits, such as sympathetic, cooperative, warm, considerate, harsh, unkind, rude. Words representing disagreeableness are reverse coded. Goldberg (1992){{cite journal

| last = Goldberg

| first = L.R.

| title = The development of markers for the Big-Five factor structure

| journal = Psychological Assessment

| year = 1992

| volume = 4

| issue = 1

| pages = 26–42

| doi = 10.1037/1040-3590.4.1.26

| s2cid = 144709415

}} developed a 20-word measure as part of his 100-word Big Five markers, and Saucier (1994){{cite journal

| last = Saucier

| first = G

| title = Mini-Markers – a brief version of Goldberg's unipolar big-five markers

| journal = Journal of Personality Assessment

| year = 1994

| volume = 63

| issue = 3

| pages = 506–516

| doi = 10.1207/s15327752jpa6303_8

| pmid = 7844738

}} developed a briefer 8-word measure as part of his 40-word mini-markers. Thompson (2008) revised these markers to develop a 40-word measure with better psychometric properties in both American and non-American populations: the International English Mini-Markers. This brief measure has good internal consistency reliabilities{{jargon inline|date=July 2023}} and other validity for assessing agreeableness and other five factor personality dimensions, both within and, especially, without American populations. Internal consistency reliability of the agreeableness measure for native English-speakers is reported as .86, that for non-native English-speakers is .80.

Statement measures tend to comprise more words, and hence consume more research instrument space, than lexical measures. Respondents are asked the extent to which they, for example, [are] on good terms with nearly everyone, [are] not interested in other people's problems or [s]ympathize with others' feelings.

Cattell's factor analytic approach, which aimed to identify {{clarify|text=universal personality structures|reason=is that a thing? is there a wikilink about it?|date=July 2023}}, inspired many studies in the decades following the introduction of the 16PF. Using Cattell's original clusters, the 16PF, and original data, multiple researchers independently developed a five factor model of personality over this period. From the early 1960s on, these explorations typically included a factor called "agreeableness" or "sociability".{{cite book

| title = Personality and Individual Differences

| publisher = Blackwell Publishing

| author = Chamorro-Premuzic, T.

| year = 2007

| location = Malden, MA

| isbn = 978-1-4051-3008-0

}} Despite repeated replications of five stable personality factors following Cattell's pioneering work, this framework only began to dominate personality research in the early 1980s with the work of Lewis Goldberg. Using lexical studies similar to those of Allport and Odbert, Goldberg chose the term "Big Five" to reflect the sheer number of personality-related terms encompassed by each of these five distinct factors. One of these, agreeableness, was defined by a number of personality-related words similar to those present in earlier and more recent manifestations of the construct; examples include "friendly", "good-natured", "cooperative", "trustful", "nurturing", "sociable", and "considerate".{{cite book

| title = Review of Personality and Social Psychology: Volume 2

| chapter = Language and individual differences: The search for universals in personality lexicons.

| publisher = Sage

| author = Goldberg, L. R.

| editor-first = L.

| editor-last = Wheeler

| year = 1981

| location = Beverly Hills, CA

| pages = 141–165

| isbn = 978-0-8039-1667-8

}}{{cite book

| title = Problems with Language Imprecision: New Directions for Methodology of Social and Behavioral Science

| url = https://archive.org/details/problemswithlang00fisk

| url-access = registration

| chapter = Developing a taxonomy of trait-descriptive terms.

| publisher = Jossey-Bass

| author = Goldberg, L. R.

| editor-first = D. W.

| editor-last = Fiske

| year = 1981

| location = San Francisco, CA

| pages = [https://archive.org/details/problemswithlang00fisk/page/43 43]–65

}}

= NEO PI =

Beginning in the 1970s, Paul Costa and Robert McCrae began researching the development of personality assessments based on factor models. Beginning with cluster analyses of Cattell's 16PF, Costa and McCrae initially settled on a three-factor model of personality. These three factors were neuroticism (vs. emotional stability), extraversion (vs. introversion), and openness (vs. closedness) to experience, resulting in the acronym "NEO."{{cite journal

| title = Age differences in personality structure: A cluster analytic approach.

| author = Costa, P. T. |author2=McCrae, R. R.

| journal = Journal of Gerontology

| year = 1976

| volume = 31

| issue = 5

| pages = 564–570

| doi = 10.1093/geronj/31.5.564

| pmid = 950450

}} Due to similarities between their three-factor NEO Personality Inventory and Goldberg's Big Five, Costa and McCrae began to develop scales to assess agreeableness and conscientiousness in the early 1980s. This work culminated in the 1985 publication of the first NEO PI Manual to be based on the full Five Factor Model.{{cite book

| title = The NEO Personality Inventory Manual.

| publisher = Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.

| author = Costa, P. T. Jr. |author2=McCrae, R. R.

| year = 1985

| location = Odessa, FL

}} Although this marked the introduction of agreeableness to the NEO PI, Costa and McCrae worked for an additional seven years to identify and elaborate on the facets comprising this factor in the Revised NEO Personality Inventory.{{cite book

| title = Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO PI-R) and the NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) Professional Manual.

| publisher = Psychological Assessment Resources

| author = Costa, P. T. |author2=McCrae, R. R.

| year = 1992

| location = Odessa, FL

}}

NEO PI facets

In the NEO PI, each of the five factors identified by Costa and McCrae are identified with six lower-level traits. Known as facets, the lower-level traits subsumed by agreeableness were first introduced with the 1992 publication of the revised version of the NEO PI. Based on the modern NEO PI-R, the six facets of agreeableness are: trust, straightforwardness, altruism, compliance, modesty, and tender-mindedness.

= Trust =

Trust is viewed to be an important feature of psychosocial development, personality theory, and folk psychological conceptions of personality.{{cite journal

| title = Facet scales for agreeableness and conscientiousness: A revision of the NEO personality inventory.

| author = Costa, P. T. |author2=McCrae, R. R.

| journal = Personality and Individual Differences

| year = 1991

| volume = 12

| issue = 9

| pages = 888

| doi = 10.1016/0191-8869(91)90177-D

}} Individuals who score high on trust generally believe others' intentions to be benevolent and can be naive if taken to the extreme. Those scoring low on this facet tend to be cynical and paranoid and view others as suspicious, dishonest, or dangerous.

= Straightforwardness =

Straightforwardness is the quality of being direct, open, and honest in communicating with others. Despite a long history in moral philosophy, straightforwardness is not as vital to personality theory as the other facets of agreeableness. Those scoring high on straightforwardness tend to interact with others in a direct and honest manner. Low scorers are less direct, tend to be high in self-monitoring, are more reticent. Those who score low on this facet also tend to be high in Machiavellianism, being deceitful or manipulative with others.{{cite journal

|author1=Jakobwitz S. |author2=Egan V. | year = 2006

| title = The dark triad and normal personality traits

| journal = Personality and Individual Differences

| volume = 40

| issue = 2

| pages = 331–339

| doi = 10.1016/j.paid.2005.07.006

}} Straightforwardness is similar to a dimension in the Interpersonal circumplex called "ingenuous versus calculating." According to Michael C. Ashton and Kibeom Lee, straightforwardness is similar to the honesty aspect of honesty-humility in the HEXACO Model.{{cite journal

|url=http://www.psy.uwa.edu.au/davidm/203/2007/honesty%20Ashton%20and%20Lee%202005.pdf

|title=Honesty-Humility, the Big Five, and the Five-Factor Model

|author=Ashton, M. C.

|author2=Lee, K.

|journal=Journal of Personality

|date=October 2005

|volume=73

|issue=5

|doi=10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00351.x

|pmid=16138875

|pages=1321–53

|url-status=dead

|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20120317204325/http://www.psy.uwa.edu.au/davidm/203/2007/honesty%20Ashton%20and%20Lee%202005.pdf

|archive-date=2012-03-17

}}

= Altruism =

Similar to altruism in animals and ethical altruism, this facet is defined by measures of selflessness, self-sacrifice, generosity, and consideration, courtesy, and concern for others. Altruism is similar to Alfred Adler's concept of social interest, which is a tendency to direct one's actions toward the betterment of society.{{cite book

| title = Superiority and Social Interest: A Collection of Later Writings

| publisher = Routledge & Kegan Paul

| author = Adler, A.

| year = 1964

| location = London

}} Individuals who score low on altruism tend to be discourteous, selfish, or greedy, a pattern of behaviors known as "self-interest" in Adlerian psychology.

= Compliance =

As a facet of agreeableness, compliance is defined as an individual's typical response to conflict. Those who score high on compliance tend to be meek and mild, and to prefer cooperation or deference as a means of resolving conflict. Low scorers tend to be aggressive, antagonistic, competitive, quarrelsome, and vindictive.

= Modesty =

While trust, straightforwardness, altruism, and compliance all refer to interpersonal or social behaviors, modesty refers to an individual's self-concept. Those who score high on modesty tend to be humble and other-focused, while low scorers tend to be arrogant, ostentatious, and self-aggrandizing. Low modesty is otherwise known as conceitedness or narcissism and, in extreme cases, can manifest as narcissistic personality disorder or histrionic personality disorder.{{cite book

| title = Comprehensive Handbook of Psychopathology: Third Edition

| publisher = Springer

| author = Adams, H. D. |author2=Sutker, P. B.

| year = 2004

| location = New York

| isbn = 978-0-306-46490-4

}} Otherwise known as "humility" in the Revised NEO Personality Inventory, modesty resembles the humility aspect of honesty-humility in the HEXACO model.

= Tender-mindedness =

Tender-mindedness is defined as the extent to which an individual's judgments and attitudes are determined by emotion. Coined by William James, this term was also prominent in early versions of the 16PF. Tender-mindedness is primarily defined by sympathy{{cite web

| url = http://www.unifr.ch/ztd/HTS/inftest/WEB-Informationssystem/en/4en001/d590668ef5a34f17908121d3edf2d1dc/hb.htm

| title = NEO-PI-R • NEO Personality Inventory – Revised

| publisher = Hogrefe Testsystem 4

| access-date = March 19, 2012

| author = Costa, P. T. |author2=McCrae, R. R

}} and corresponds to the International Personality Item Pool's "sympathy" scale.{{cite web

| url = https://ipip.ori.org/newNEO_FacetsTable.htm

| title = A Comparison between the 30 Facet Scales in Costa and McCrae's NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and the Corresponding Preliminary IPIP Scales Measuring Similar Constructs

| publisher = Oregon Research Institute

| access-date = March 19, 2012

}} In contrast, "tough minded" is a trait associated with Psychoticism on the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire.{{cite book

| title = Manual of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire

| publisher = Educational and Industrial Testing Service.

| author = Eysenck, H. J. |author2=Eysenck, S. B. G.

| year = 1975

| location = San Diego

}}

Equivalents in psychobiological models

Models based on psychobiological theories of personality have each incorporated a factor similar to agreeableness. In Cloninger's Temperament and Character Inventory the character trait known as cooperativeness is very similar to and positively correlated with agreeableness.{{cite journal

| last = De Fruyt

| first = F.

| author2 = Van De Wiele, L.

| author3 = Van Heeringen, C.

| title = Cloninger's Psychobiological Model of Temperament and Character and the Five-Factor Model of Personality

| journal = Personality and Individual Differences

| year = 2000

| volume = 29

| pages = 441–452

| doi = 10.1016/S0191-8869(99)00204-4

| issue = 3

}} In Zuckerman's alternative five model of personality the trait known as aggression-hostility is inversely related to agreeableness.{{cite journal

| doi = 10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00186-6

| title = A comparative study of Zuckerman's three structural models for personality through the NEO-PI-R, ZKPQ-III-R, EPQ-RS and Goldberg's 50-bipolar adjectives

| year = 2002

| last1 = Aluja

| first1 = Anton

| last2 = García

| first2 = Óscar

| last3 = García

| first3 = Luis F.

| journal = Personality and Individual Differences

| volume = 33

| issue = 5

| pages = 713–725

}}

HEXACO model

To address the absence of measures of dark triad traits (i.e., narcissism, machiavellianism, and psychopathy), Michael Ashton and Kibeom Lee proposed the addition of a sixth factor to the five factor model.{{cite journal

| title = Honesty as the sixth factor of personality: Correlations with Machiavellianism, primary psychopathy, and social adroitness

| author1 = Ashton, M. C. |author2=Lee, K. |author3=Son, C

| journal = European Journal of Personality

| year = 2000

| volume = 14

| issue = 4

| doi = 10.1002/1099-0984(200007/08)14:4<359::AID-PER382>3.0.CO;2-Y

| pages=359–368

| s2cid = 144035257 }} Validated with psycholexical studies similar to those used in the development of the five factor model,{{cite journal

| title = A six-factor structure of personality-descriptive adjectives: Solutions from psycholexical studies in seven languages

| author1 = Ashton, M. C. |author2=Lee, K. |author3=Perugini, M. |author4=Szarota, P. |author5=de Vries, R. E. |author6=Di Blas, L. |author7=Boies, K. |author8=De Raad, B. |display-authors=3

| journal = Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

| date = February 2004

| volume = 86

| issue = 2

| doi = 10.1037/0022-3514.86.2.356

| pmid = 14769090

| pages = 356–66

}} the HEXACO model adds honesty-humility to five factors resembling those in the NEO PI.{{cite journal

| title = The HEXACO Personality Inventory: A new measure of the major dimensions of personality

|author1=Lee, K. |author2=Ashton, M. C.

| journal = Multivariate Behavioral Research

| year = 2004

| volume = 39

}} Although honesty-humility does not directly correspond to any Big Five trait, it is strongly correlated with the straightforwardness and modesty facets of Big Five agreeableness. As both of these facets are only weakly correlated with Big Five agreeableness, Ashton and Lee suggest dividing NEO PI agreeableness into two factors similar to those in the HEXACO model: honesty-humility (i.e., straightforwardness and modesty) and a redefined agreeableness (trust, altruism, compliance, and tender-mindedness). Reflecting this conception of honesty-humility and HEXACO agreeableness as unique though similar concepts, Ashton and Lee propose that they represent different aspects of reciprocal altruism: fairness (honesty-humility) and tolerance (agreeableness).{{cite journal

| title = Empirical, theoretical, and practical advantages of the HEXACO Model of Personality Structure.

| author = Ashton, M. C. |author2=Lee, K.

| journal = Personality and Social Psychology Review

| date = May 2007

| volume = 11

| issue = 2

| pages = 150–66

| doi = 10.1177/1088868306294907

| pmid = 18453460

| s2cid = 13183244 }}

Despite suggesting this reconceptualization of agreeableness for the NEO PI, Ashton and Lee do not believe HEXACO agreeableness is accurately captured by trust, altruism, compliance, and tender-mindedness. In addition to accounting for these four facets of Big Five agreeableness, the HEXACO model's construction of agreeableness includes content categorized under neuroticism in the NEO PI (i.e., temperamentalness and irritability).{{cite journal

|url=http://www.psy.uwa.edu.au/davidm/203/2007/Psyc%20Props%20lee%20and%20ashton%202004.pdf

|title=Psychometric properties of the HEXACO Personality Inventory.

|author=Lee, K.

|author2=Ashton, M. C.

|journal=Multivariate Behavioral Research

|year=2004

|volume=39

|issue=2

|doi=10.1207/s15327906mbr3902_8

|pmid=26804579

|pages=329–358

|s2cid=27763606

|url-status=dead

|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20120317220126/http://www.psy.uwa.edu.au/davidm/203/2007/Psyc%20Props%20lee%20and%20ashton%202004.pdf

|archive-date=2012-03-17

}} To reflect the negative emotional content at the low end of HEXACO agreeableness, this factor is also referred to as "agreeableness (versus anger)." The inclusion of anger in the definition of HEXACO agreeableness further helps to differentiate this factor from honesty-humility. In response to offensive or transgressive actions, individuals who score low on honesty-humility tend not to respond immediately. Instead, they defer their response by planning their revenge and waiting for the perfect opportunity to enact it. Although those who score low on HEXACO agreeableness also employ this premeditated strategy, they also tend to respond immediately with anger.{{cite journal

| title = Getting mad and getting even: Agreeableness and Honesty-Humility as predictors of revenge intentions.

| author = Lee, K. |author2=Ashton, M. C.

| journal = Personality and Individual Differences

| date = April 2012

| volume = 52

| issue = 5

| doi = 10.1016/j.paid.2011.12.004

| pages=596–600

| s2cid = 14026546 }}

= HEXACO agreeableness facets =

To help capture the numerous distinctions between the Big Five and HEXACO models, Ashton and Lee propose four new facet labels in their conceptualization of agreeableness: forgiveness, gentleness, flexibility, and patience. In addition to these four Agreeableness-specific facets, Lee and Ashton have proposed an additional "interstitial" facet located in a space shared by agreeableness, honesty-humility, and emotionality: altruism versus antagonism.{{cite journal

| title = Further assessment of the HEXACO Personality Inventory: Two new facet scales and an observer report form.

| author = Lee, K. |author2=Ashton, M. C.

| journal = Psychological Assessment

| date = June 2006

| volume = 18

| issue = 2

| doi = 10.1037/1040-3590.18.2.182

| pmid = 16768594

| pages = 182–91

}}

  • Forgiveness: A measure of an individual's response to deception or other transgressions. Individuals who score high on this facet tend to regain their trust and re-establish friendly relations by forgiving the offender, while those who score low tend to hold a grudge. Also known as "forgivingness".{{cite web

| url = https://hexaco.org/scaledescriptions.html

| title = The HEXACO Personality Inventory-Revised: Scale Descriptions

| access-date = March 20, 2012

| author = Lee, K. |author2=Ashton, M. C.

}}

  • Gentleness: A measure of how an individual typically evaluates others. Individuals who score high on this facet tend to avoid being overly judgmental, while those who score low are highly critical and judgmental.
  • Flexibility: A measure of behaviors related to compromise and cooperation. Individuals who score high on this facet prefer cooperation and compromise as means of resolving disagreement, while those who score low tend to be stubborn, argumentative, and unwilling to accommodate others.
  • Patience: A measure of one's response to anger and aggravation. Individuals who score high on this facet tend to be able to tolerate very high levels of anger and maintain their composure while expressing anger. Those who score low on patience have difficulties remaining calm while expressing their anger and tend to have quick tempers, becoming very angry in response to comparatively little provocation.
  • Altruism versus antagonism: Although shared between three HEXACO factors, altruism versus antagonism is moderately correlated with agreeableness. This interstitial facet assesses the extent to which an individual is sympathetic, soft-hearted, and helpful, with low-scoring individuals tending toward an antagonistic interpersonal style.

Interpersonal relations

Agreeableness is an asset in situations that require getting along with others. Compared to disagreeable persons, agreeable individuals display a tendency to perceive others in a more positive light.

Because agreeable children are more sensitive to the needs and perspectives of others, they are less likely to suffer from social rejection. Children who are less disruptive, less aggressive, and more skilled at entering play groups are more likely to gain acceptance by their peers.{{cite book|last=Bierman|first=K.L.|year=2003|title=Peer rejection: Developmental processes and intervention strategies|location=New York|publisher=The Guilford Press}}

One study found that people high in agreeableness are more {{clarify|text=emotionally responsive|reason=what does it mean to be "emotionally responsive"?|date=July 2023}} in social situations. This effect was measured on both self-report questionnaires and physiological measures, and offers evidence that extraversion and neuroticism are not the only Big Five personality factors that influence emotion. The effect was especially pronounced among women.{{cite journal

|author1=Tobin R.M. |author2=Graziano W.G. |author3=Vanman E. |author4=Tassinary L. | year = 2000

| title = Personality, emotional experience, and efforts to control emotions

| journal = Journal of Personality & Social Psychology

| volume = 79

| issue = 4

| pages = 656–669

| doi = 10.1037/0022-3514.79.4.656

|pmid=11045745 }}

Research also shows that people high in agreeableness are more likely to control negative emotions like anger in conflict situations. Those who are high in agreeableness are more likely to use conflict-avoidant tactics when in conflict with others (whereas people low in agreeableness are more likely to use coercive tactics, like stonewalling or shunning).{{cite journal

|author1=Jensen-Campbell L. A. |author2=Graziano W. G. | year = 2001

| title = Agreeableness as a moderator of interpersonal conflict

| journal = Journal of Personality

| volume = 69

| issue = 2

| pages = 323–361

| doi = 10.1111/1467-6494.00148

|pmid=11339802 |s2cid=801327 }} They are also more willing to give ground to their adversary and may lose arguments with people who are less agreeable. From their perspective, they have not really lost an argument as much as maintained a congenial relationship with another person.{{cite journal

|author1=Graziano W.G. |author2=Jensen-Campbell L.A. |author3=Hair E. C. | year = 1996

| title = Perceiving interpersonal conflict and reacting to it: The case for agreeableness

| journal = Journal of Personality & Social Psychology

| volume = 70

| issue = 4

| pages = 820–835

| doi = 10.1037/0022-3514.70.4.820

|pmid=8636901 }}

Prosocial behaviour

Agreeableness is positively associated with altruism and helping behaviour. Across situations, people who are high in agreeableness are more likely to report an interest and involvement with helping others. Experiments have shown that most people are likely to help their own kin, and to help when empathy has been aroused. Agreeable people are likely to help even when these conditions are not present.{{cite journal|last1=Graziano|first1=W.G.|last2=Habashi|first2=M.M.|last3=Sheese|first3=B.E.|last4=Tobin|first4=R.M.|year=2007|title=Agreeableness, empathy, and helping: A person X situation perspective|journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology|volume=93 |issue=4 |pages=583–599 |doi=10.1037/0022-3514.93.4.583 |pmid=17892333 }} In other words, agreeable people appear to be "traited for helping"{{cite journal

|author1=Penner L. A. |author2=Fritzsche B. A. |author3=Craiger J. P. |author4=Freifeld T. S. | year = 1995

| title = Measuring the prosocial personality

| journal = Advances in Personality Assessment

| volume = 10

| pages = 147–163

}} and do not need any other motivations.

While agreeable individuals are habitually likely to help others, disagreeable people may be more likely to cause harm. Researchers have found that low levels of agreeableness are associated with hostile thoughts and aggression in adolescents, as well as poor social adjustment.{{cite journal

|author1=Gleason K.A. |author2=Jensen-Campbell L.A. |author3=Richardson D. | year = 2004

| title = Agreeableness and aggression in adolescence

| journal = Aggressive Behavior

| volume = 30

| pages = 43–61

| doi = 10.1002/ab.20002

}} People low in agreeableness are also more likely to be prejudiced against stigmatized groups such as the overweight.{{cite journal

|author1=Graziano W.G. |author2=Bruce J. W. |author3=Sheese B.E. |author4=Tobin R.M. | year = 2007

| title = Attraction, personality and prejudice: Liking none of the people most of the time

| journal = Journal of Personality & Social Psychology

| volume = 93

| issue = 4

| pages = 565–582

| doi = 10.1037/0022-3514.93.4.565

|pmid=17892332 }}

However, high agreeableness does not always lead to prosocial behaviour. In a Milgram experiment, conscientious and agreeable people, when advised by an ill-intending authority, are more willing to administer high-intensity electric shocks to a victim, because conscientious and agreeable people are less capable of resistance.{{cite journal|last1=Bègue|first1=Laurent|last2=Beauvois|first2=Jean-Léon|last3=Courbert|first3=Didier|last4=Oberblé|first4=Dominique|last5=Lepage|first5=Johan|last6=Duke|first6=Aaron|title=Personality Predicts Obedience in a Milgram Paradigm|journal=Journal of Personality|volume=83|issue=3|pages=299–306|date=2014|doi=10.1111/jopy.12104|pmid=24798990|s2cid=13868371 |url=https://archivesic.ccsd.cnrs.fr/sic_01158395/file/Begue%2C%20Beauvois%2CCourbet%20J%20of%20personality%202015%20paru.pdf}}

Intelligence

Agreeableness and its related traits have generally been assumed to be uncorrelated with cognitive abilities. In general, it does have the fewest and smallest connections with intelligence. However, large-scale meta-analyses have revealed that the aspects of agreeableness (i.e., compassion and politeness) have meaningful and opposite relations with cognitive abilities. For example, compassion correlates .26 with general mental ability whereas politeness correlates -.22 with general science knowledge.{{Cite journal |last1=Stanek |first1=Kevin C. |last2=Ones |first2=Deniz S. |date=2023-06-06 |title=Meta-analytic relations between personality and cognitive ability |journal=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences |language=en |volume=120 |issue=23 |pages=e2212794120 |doi=10.1073/pnas.2212794120 |doi-access=free |issn=0027-8424 |pmc=10266031 |pmid=37252971|bibcode=2023PNAS..12012794S }} Facets of agreeableness also demonstrate some meaningful connections with various cognitive abilities (e.g., cooperation and processing speed correlate .20, modesty and ideational fluency correlate -.17).{{Cite book |last1=Stanek |first1=Kevin |url=https://umnlibraries.manifoldapp.org/projects/of-anchors-and-sails |title=Of Anchors & Sails: Personality-Ability Trait Constellations |last2=Ones |first2=Deniz |date=2023-11-20 |publisher=University of Minnesota |isbn=978-1-946135-98-8 |doi=10.24926/9781946135988|s2cid=265335858 }}

From childhood to adulthood

Agreeableness is important to psychological well-being, predicting mental health, positive affect, and good relations with others. Both childhood and adolescent agreeableness have predictive value. Along with this it has also been implicated to conflict management skills, school adjustment, peer-social status, and self-esteem. Among young adults, individuals that have been diagnosed with externalizing as well as internalizing disorders present lower levels of agreeableness and communion, and higher levels of negative emotionality, than those young adults without such disorders. Disorders such as major depressive disorder negatively correlate to levels of agreeableness. {{Cite journal |last=Wu |first=Pei-Chen |date=2023-02-01 |title=Associations Between Personality and Depressive Symptoms in an Adolescent Clinical Population: Consideration of Personality Stability |url=https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s10578-021-01227-1 |journal=Child Psychiatry & Human Development |language=en |volume=54 |issue=1 |pages=84–95 |doi=10.1007/s10578-021-01227-1 |pmid=34374887 |issn=1573-3327|url-access=subscription }} Agreeableness also is reported anger and depression in young adults.{{Cite journal |last1=Ode |first1=Scott |last2=Robinson |first2=Michael D. |date=April 2009 |title=Can Agreeableness Turn Gray Skies Blue? A Role for Agreeableness in Moderating Neuroticism-Linked Dysphoria |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2009.28.4.436 |journal=Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology |volume=28 |issue=4 |pages=436–462 |doi=10.1521/jscp.2009.28.4.436 |issn=0736-7236|url-access=subscription }} Across adulthood, low agreeableness has been found to be a health risk. High agreeableness, especially trust and honesty, has been linked to longevity.{{cite journal

|author1=Laursen B. |author2=Pulkkinen L. |author3=Adams R. | year = 2002

| title = The antecedents and correlates of agreeableness in adulthood

| journal = Journal of Developmental Psychology

| volume = 38

| issue = 4

| pages = 591–603

| doi = 10.1037/0012-1649.38.4.591

|pmid=12090488 |pmc=2730208}}

A study done by Caspi, Elder, and Bem (1987) found that explosive and ill-tempered children had higher rates of divorce as adults when compared with their even-tempered peers. Further, ill-tempered men had lower educational attainment, occupational status, and work stability, and ill-tempered women married men with similar low achievement profiles.{{cite journal

|author1=Caspi A. |author2=Elder G. H. |author3=Bem D. J. | year = 1987

| title = Moving against the world: Life course patterns of explosive children

| journal = Journal of Developmental Psychology

| volume = 23

| issue = 2

| pages = 308–313

| doi = 10.1037/0012-1649.23.2.308

}} A second and more recent study by Shiner (2000) found that composite variables describing middle-childhood agreeableness and friendly compliance predicted adolescent academic performance, behavioral conduct, and social competence ten years later.{{cite journal

| author = Shiner R. L.

| year = 2000

| title = Linking childhood personality with adaptation: evidence for continuity and change across time into late adolescence

| journal = Journal of Personality & Social Psychology

| volume = 78

| issue = 2

| pages = 310–325

| doi = 10.1037/0022-3514.78.2.310

| pmid = 10707337

}}

Some personality traits, such as openness,{{cite journal | vauthors = Grossman AB, Levin BE, Bradley WG | title = Premorbid personality characteristics of patients with ALS | journal = Amyotrophic Lateral Sclerosis | volume = 7 | issue = 1 | pages = 27–31 | date = March 2006 | pmid = 16546756 | doi = 10.1080/14660820510012004 | s2cid = 20998807 }} agreeableness{{cite journal | vauthors = Parkin Kullmann JA, Hayes S, Pamphlett R | title = Are people with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS) particularly nice? An international online case-control study of the Big Five personality factors | journal = Brain and Behavior | volume = 8 | issue = 10 | pages = e01119 | date = October 2018 | pmid = 30239176 | pmc = 6192405 | doi = 10.1002/brb3.1119 }} and conscientiousness appear remarkably common among patients with Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS). It is not clear whether personality can increase susceptibility to ALS directly.{{cite journal | vauthors = Mehl T, Jordan B, Zierz S | title = "Patients with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS) are usually nice persons"-How physicians experienced in ALS see the personality characteristics of their patients | journal = Brain and Behavior | volume = 7 | issue = 1 | pages = e00599 | date = January 2017 | pmid = 28127517 | pmc = 5256182 | doi = 10.1002/brb3.599 }} Instead, genetic factors giving rise to personality might simultaneously predispose people to develop ALS, or the above personality traits might underlie lifestyle choices which are in turn risk factors for ALS.

Geography

=United States=

File:Agreeableness by state.jpg

In the United States, people in the West, Midwest, and South tend to have higher average scores on agreeableness than people living in other regions.{{cite web

|url=http://mypersonality.org/research/2008/12/09/the-relationship-between-us-state-and-personality/

|title=The relationship between US state and personality.

|publisher=myPersonality Research

|date=December 9, 2008

|access-date=April 7, 2012

|url-status=dead

|archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20120318164923/http://mypersonality.org/research/2008/12/09/the-relationship-between-us-state-and-personality/

|archive-date=March 18, 2012

}} According to researchers, the top ten most agreeable states are North Dakota, Minnesota, Mississippi, Utah, Wisconsin, Tennessee, North Carolina, Georgia, Oklahoma, and Nebraska.

{{cite news

| url = https://www.wsj.com/articles/SB122211987961064719?mod=yhoofront

| title = The United States of Mind. Researchers Identify Regional Personality Traits Across America

| author = Stephanie Simon

| work = WSJ.com

| date = 2008-09-23

}} Original research article:

{{cite journal |author1=Peter J. Rentfrow |author2=Samuel D. Gosling |author3=Jeff Potter |title=A Theory of the Emergence, Persistence, and Expression of Geographic Variation in Psychological Characteristics |journal=Perspectives on Psychological Science |volume=3 |issue=5 |pages=339–369 |year=2008 |url=https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00084.x |doi=10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00084.x |pmid=26158954 |s2cid=17059908 |url-access=subscription }} These findings are consistent with well-known expressions in these states, such as "southern hospitality" and "Minnesota nice." Because these states are generally less urbanized than the east and west coasts, people may be more likely to live in small communities and know their neighbors. Consequently, they may be more willing to care about and help their neighbours.

In a study done by Albright et al. (1997) groups of college students from China and the United States rated strangers from both countries on the "Big Five" personality traits, external traits, and how well they were dressed. They found that both Chinese and U.S. students rated faces as showing similar levels of agreeableness and extroversion. The people who were thought to be the most agreeable wore smiles, a facial expression that is recognized around the world.{{cite book |title=Psychology |publisher=The McGraw-Hill Companies. |author1=C. B. Wortman |author2=E. F. Loftus |author3=C. A. Weaver |year=1999 |url=http://www.mhhe.com/socscience/intro/cafe/wort/olc/18466.htm |access-date=2012-03-29 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20160318145628/http://www.mhhe.com/socscience/intro/cafe/wort/olc/18466.htm |archive-date=2016-03-18 |url-status=dead }} The findings seem to suggest that the trait of agreeableness is attributed to people in a universal way.{{Cite journal|last1=Albright|first1=Linda|last2=Malloy|first2=Thomas E.|last3=Dong|first3=Qi|last4=Kenny|first4=David A.|last5=Fang|first5=Xiaoyi|last6=Winquist|first6=Lynn|last7=Yu|first7=Da|date=1997|title=Cross-cultural consensus in personality judgments.|journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology|language=en|volume=72|issue=3|pages=558–569|doi=10.1037/0022-3514.72.3.558|pmid=9120784 |issn=1939-1315}}

See also

{{clear}}

References