topographical disorientation
{{Short description|Inability to orient oneself in one's surroundings}}
{{broader|Spatial disorientation}}
Topographical disorientation is the inability to orient oneself in one's surroundings, sometimes as a result of focal brain damage.{{cite journal |vauthors=Aguirre GK, D'Esposito M |title=Topographical disorientation: a synthesis and taxonomy |journal=Brain |volume=122 |issue= 9|pages=1613–28 |date=September 1999 |pmid=10468502 |doi=10.1093/brain/122.9.1613 |doi-access=free }} This disability may result from the inability to make use of selective spatial information (e.g., environmental landmarks) or to orient by means of specific cognitive strategies such as the ability to form a mental representation of the environment, also known as a cognitive map. It may be part of a syndrome known as visuospatial dysgnosia.
Classification
Topographical disorientation is the inability to find one's way through an environment due to cognitive impairment.
Topographical disorientation has been studied for decades using case studies of patients who have selectively lost their ability to find their way within large-scale, locomotor environments. Several dozen case reports of topographical disorientation have been presented over the last century. Studying these people will aid in the understanding of the complex, multi-component behavior of navigation. Topographical disorientation may be a lifelong deficit, it may result from a stroke, or it can occur as part of a progressive illness. Frequently comorbid with this disorder are hemispatial neglect, achromatopsia, prosopagnosia, and Alzheimer's disease.{{citation needed|date=August 2021}}
=Developmental=
Developmental topographical disorientation (DTD) refers to the inability to orient from childhood despite the absence of any apparent brain damage, neurological condition or general cognitive defects. Individuals affected by DTD are unable to generate a mental representation of the environment (i.e. a cognitive map) and therefore unable to make use of it while orienting (a process that usually people go through while orienting). Not to be confused with healthy individuals who have a poor sense of direction, individuals affected by DTD get lost in very familiar surroundings, such as their house or neighborhood, daily.{{cite journal |vauthors=Iaria G, Bogod N, Fox CJ, Barton JJ |title=Developmental topographical disorientation: case one |journal=Neuropsychologia |volume=47 |issue=1 |pages=30–40 |date=January 2009 |pmid=18793658 |doi=10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.08.021 |s2cid=207235010 |url=http://www.neurolab.ca/2009(3)_Iaria.pdf}}{{cite journal |vauthors=Bianchini F, Incoccia C, Palermo L, etal |title=Developmental topographical disorientation in a healthy subject |journal=Neuropsychologia |volume=48 |issue=6 |pages=1563–73 |date=May 2010 |pmid=20144632 |doi=10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.01.025 |s2cid=24124970 |url=http://www.neurolab.ca/2009(6)_Incoccia.pdf}}
3% of the sample had DTD in a study of 1,698 Italians aged between 18 and 35 years (to exclude people with cognitive decline). It was more prevalent in males than females, although, in general, males use more complex navigational strategies. The sense of direction was closely related to gender, navigational strategies adopted, and town knowledge.{{cite journal |author=Laura Piccardi, Massimiliano Palmiero, Vincenza Cofini, Paola Verde, Maddalena Boccia, Liana Palermo, Cecilia Guariglia, Raffaella Nori |date=July 20, 2022
|title="Where am I?" A snapshot of the developmental topographical disorientation among young Italian adults |journal=PLOS ONE|volume=17
|issue=7
|pages=e0271334
|doi=10.1371/journal.pone.0271334
|pmid=35857777
|pmc=9299294
|bibcode=2022PLoSO..1771334P
|doi-access=free
}}
=Egocentric=
Egocentric disorientation is marked by the inability to represent the location of objects with respect to self. This is usually due to lesions in the posterior parietal lobe. Patients experience no difficulty recognizing or naming people or objects. They are unable to accurately reach for visual objects and are unable to state the relationship between objects and oneself (above, below, left, right, nearer or farther).
In a case study presented by Stark and colleagues, a patient named GW described the inability to accurately reach for visual targets despite normal vision. She had no difficulty recognizing and naming objects presented to her, but was unable to point to locations of targets defined by visual, proprioceptive, or audio input. The loss of an egocentric spatial representation system left her unable to position herself in space. Most indicative of her disability is that she often turned in the wrong direction when greeted by someone who she was not facing.{{cite journal
| last1 =Stark
| first1 =M
| last2=Coslett
| first2=HB
| last3=Saffran
|first3=EM
| year =1996
| title = Impairment of an egocentric map of locations: implications for perception and action
| journal =Cognitive Neuropsychology
| volume = 13
| issue = 4
| pages =481–523
| doi=10.1080/026432996381908
}}
=Heading=
Heading disorientation is marked by the inability to represent direction of orientation with respect to external environment. This is usually due to lesions in the posterior cingulate. Patients show no signs of visuo-spatial agnosia. Patients are able to determine their location using landmarks, but are unable to determine which direction to proceed from those landmarks in order to reach their destination. They are also impaired in map drawing tasks and are unable to describe routes between familiar locations.
Takahashi and colleagues presented three cases of focal brain damage to the right retrosplenial region through a cerebral hemorrhage that caused a loss in sense of direction. All three patients showed normal visual perception, were able to identify familiar buildings and landscapes, were able to determine and remember locations of objects that could be seen from where they were standing, but were unable to recall direction from selective familiar landmarks. Symptoms of topographical disorientation disappeared in all three patients after two months.{{cite journal |vauthors=Takahashi N, Kawamura M, Shiota J, Kasahata N, Hirayama K |title=Pure topographic disorientation due to right posterior cingulate lesion |journal=Neurology |volume=49 |issue=2 |pages=464–9 |date=August 1997 |pmid=9270578 |doi=10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70801-3 |s2cid=4483900 }}
=Anterograde amnesiac=
Anterograde disorientation is marked by the inability to orient in new environments. This is usually due to lesions in the parahippocampus. Patients were able to navigate through and draw maps of environments learned at least 6 months before the damage.{{cite journal |author=Ross ED |title=Sensory-specific and fractional disorders of recent memory in man. I. Isolated loss of visual recent memory |journal=Arch. Neurol. |volume=37 |issue=4 |pages=193–200 |date=April 1980 |pmid=7362483 |doi= 10.1001/archneur.1980.00500530031001}} Teng and colleagues tested a profoundly amnesic patient who has complete bilateral damage to the hippocampus and extensive damage to adjacent structures in the medial temporal lobe. The patient was able to recall the spatial layout of his hometown where he moved away from 50 years ago, before he became amnesic. The patient has no knowledge of his current neighborhood, where he moved to after his injury. The finding indicates that the medial temporal lobe is not needed for the retrieval of spatial maps learned prior to injury. The hippocampus and other surrounding structures are essential for the formation of long-term declarative memories, including spatial memories.{{cite journal |vauthors=Teng E, Squire LR |title=Memory for places learned long ago is intact after hippocampal damage |journal=Nature |volume=400 |issue=6745 |pages=675–7 |date=August 1999 |pmid=10458163 |doi=10.1038/23276 |bibcode=1999Natur.400..675T |s2cid=204995252 |url=http://whoville.ucsd.edu/PDFs/309_Teng_Squire_Nature1999.pdf}}{{Cite journal |last1=Roseman |first1=Moshe |last2=Elias |first2=Uri |last3=Kletenik |first3=Isaiah |last4=Ferguson |first4=Michael A. |last5=Fox |first5=Michael D. |last6=Horowitz |first6=Zalman |last7=Marshall |first7=Gad A. |last8=Spiers |first8=Hugo J. |last9=Arzy |first9=Shahar |date=2023-12-13 |title=A neural circuit for spatial orientation derived from brain lesions |journal=Cerebral Cortex |volume=34 |issue=1 |pages=bhad486 |doi=10.1093/cercor/bhad486 |issn=1460-2199 |pmid=38100330|pmc=10793567 }}
=Topographagnosia=
Landmark agnosia, also known as topographical agnosia and topographagnosia, is marked by the inability to recognize salient environmental stimuli such as landmarks. This is usually due to lesions in the lingual gyrus. Patients are able to draw detailed maps and visualize places familiar to them before the illness. They can distinguish between classes of buildings, such as house or skyscraper, but are unable to identify specific buildings, such as their own house or famous landmarks. Patients can navigate using strictly spatial information and specific details of landmarks such as house number or door color.
C. A. Pallis described a patient, A.H., who presented with color, face and landmark agnosia as a result of a cerebral embolism.{{Citation
| last =Pallis
| first =CA
| year =1955
| title = Impaired identification of faces and places with agnosia for colours
| journal =Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry
| volume = 18
| issue =3
| pages =218–24
|pmc=503242
| pmid=13252431
| doi =10.1136/jnnp.18.3.218
}}
Diagnosis
Topographical disorientation is usually diagnosed with the use of a comprehensive battery of neuropsychological tests combined with a variety of orientation tasks performed by the participants in both virtual and real surroundings. Performance on certain tests can identify underlying neurological disorders and verify the disorientation as a selective impairment. Brain imaging is used to determine regions of brain damage, if any. Navigational skills can be assessed by tests pertaining to memory, visual-perceptual abilities, object recognition, mental rotation, imagery abilities, and spatial abilities. More direct testing of navigation involves asking the patient to describe a route, read a map, draw a map, follow a route, or point out landmarks.
Treatment
Treatment for topographical disorientation has been achieved through a case by case basis. Prognosis is largely dependent on the organic cause. Neuropsychological assessment followed by an assessment of unaffected cognitive abilities can be employed in therapy. Treatment for recovering navigational skills requires strengthening unaffected navigational strategies to bypass any defective ones.{{citation needed|date=August 2021}}
See also
References
{{reflist|30em}}
Further reading
- {{cite journal |vauthors=Aguirre GK, Zarahn E, D'Esposito M |title=Neural components of topographical representation |journal=Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. |volume=95 |issue=3 |pages=839–46 |date=February 1998 |pmid=9448249 |pmc=33806 |doi= 10.1073/pnas.95.3.839|bibcode=1998PNAS...95..839A |doi-access=free }}
- {{cite journal |author=Antonakos CL |title=Compensatory wayfinding behavior in topographic disorientation from brain injury |journal=Journal of Environmental Psychology |volume=24 |issue=4 |pages=495–502 |year=2004 |doi=10.1016/j.jenvp.2004.09.002 }}
- {{cite journal |vauthors=Brunsdon R, Nickels L, Coltheart M |title=Topographical disorientation: towards an integrated framework for assessment |journal=Neuropsychol Rehabil |volume=17 |issue=1 |pages=34–52 |date=January 2007 |pmid=17178604 |doi=10.1080/09602010500505021 |s2cid=29766248 }}
- {{cite book |author=Paul Dudchenko |title=Why people get lost: the psychology and neuroscience of spatial cognition |publisher=Oxford University Press |location=Oxford [Oxfordshire] |year=2010 |isbn=978-0-19-921086-2 |oclc=791205815 }}
- {{cite journal |vauthors=Kirshner HS, Lavin PJ |title=Posterior cortical atrophy: a brief review |journal=Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep |volume=6 |issue=6 |pages=477–80 |date=November 2006 |pmid=17074282 |doi= 10.1007/s11910-006-0049-0|s2cid=44713747 }}
- {{cite journal |vauthors=Lim TS, Iaria G, Moon SY |title=Topographical disorientation in mild cognitive impairment: a voxel-based morphometry study |journal=J Clin Neurol |volume=6 |issue=4 |pages=204–11 |date=December 2010 |pmid=21264201 |pmc=3024525 |doi=10.3988/jcn.2010.6.4.204 }}
- {{cite book |author=Jonsson, Erik |title=Inner navigation: why we get lost and how we find our way |publisher=Scribner |location=New York |year=2002 |isbn=978-0-7432-2206-8 |oclc=48579029 |url-access=registration |url=https://archive.org/details/innernavigationw00jons }}
- {{cite journal |author=Takahashi N |title=[Agnosia for streets and defective root finding] |language=ja |journal=Brain Nerve |volume=63 |issue=8 |pages=830–8 |date=August 2011 |pmid=21817174 }}
- {{cite journal |vauthors=Wilson BA, Berry E, Gracey F, etal |title=Egocentric disorientation following bilateral parietal lobe damage |journal=Cortex |volume=41 |issue=4 |pages=547–54 |date=August 2005 |pmid=16042030 |doi= 10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70194-1|s2cid=4478452 }}
External links
- [http://nymag.com/scienceofus/2014/11/what-its-like-to-feel-lost-in-your-own-home.html What It's Like to Feel Lost in Your Own Home]
- [https://www.gettinglost.ca Where to Learn More about DTD and Take some Online Tests]