Avava language
{{short description|Austronesian language spoken in Vanuatu}}
{{nf|date=June 2019}}
{{Infobox language
|name=Avava
|altname=Katbol
|region=Central Malekula
|state=Vanuatu
|speakers=700
|date=2001
|ref=e18
|familycolor=Austronesian
|fam2=Malayo-Polynesian
|fam3=Oceanic
|fam4=Southern Oceanic
|fam5=North-Central Vanuatu
|fam6=Central Vanuatu
|fam7=Malakula
|fam8=Malakula Interior
|iso3=tmb
|glotto=katb1237
|glottorefname=Avava
|map = Lang Status 99-NE.svg
|mapcaption = {{center|{{small|Avava is not endangered according to the classification system of the UNESCO Atlas of the World's Languages in Danger}}}}
}}
Avava (Navava), also known as Katbol, Tembimbe-Katbol, or Bangsa’ is an Oceanic language of central Malekula, Vanuatu. It has nasalized fricatives and a bilabial trill.
The four Avava-speaking villages speak or spoke, distinct dialects. Timbembe and Nevaar (Nɨviar) are still spoken. The Nivat (Nevat) and Bangasa (Umbrul) dialects are extinct. Bangasa/Bangsa', or more correctly Bangasak, was known as Numbuwul by its neighbors to the north; the endonym is Umbbuul {{IPA|tmb|(u)ᵐʙuːl|}}.
Names
The alternate names for Avava are Bangsa’, Katbol, Mallicolo, Navava, Taremp, Tembimbe-Katbol and Tisvel.{{Cite web |title=Glottolog 5.1 - Avava |url=https://glottolog.org/resource/languoid/id/katb1237 |access-date=2025-02-13 |website=glottolog.org}}
Phonology
When the final syllable is light (CV), stress tends to be penultimate. When the final syllable is heavy (CVC, CVV, CVː), stress tends to be final.
=Vowels=
There are a total of eight vowel quantities in Avava: five short vowels and three long vowels. The five short Avava vowel qualities, {{IPA|/a e i o u/}}. {{IPA|/u/}} is pronounced {{IPA|[ʉ]}} between a bilabial trill and an alveolar and, in final syllables, between a bilabial trill and {{IPA|/k/}}. About 2% of vowels are long. Long {{IPA|/eː/}} is not attested, and long {{IPA|/oː/}} is marginal. This is a pattern shared with Naman. At the end of a prosodic unit – in citation form, utterance-finally and when speaking slowly – word-final vowels other than {{IPA|/i/}} tend to be replaced with "diphthongs" {{IPA|/Vi/}}. Word-initial vowels present in citation form tend to be lost when the word is linked to others, e.g. when the subject of a verb or possessed by a pronoun. This is the reason for the alternative form of the name of the language, vava.
:
class="wikitable" style="text-align:center"
! ! front ! back |
high
| {{IPA link|i}} | {{IPA link|u}} |
---|
mid
| {{IPA link|e}} | {{IPA link|o}} |
low
| colspan="2"| {{IPA link|a}} |
A notable variant of the same phoneme shown with short vowels is when /u/ undergoes centralisation to [ʉ] in two different settings: in closed syllables between a bilabial trill and a following alveolar consonant, and {{clarify|text=in non-final syllables between a bilabial trill and alveolar consonant|date=October 2019}}.
The three long vowels in Avava are /i:/, /u:/, and /a:/. Though there is evidence for the long /o:/, the vowel is only shown in three words throughout the entire lexicon of Avava.
=Consonants=
:
class="wikitable" style="text-align:center"
|+ Avava consonant inventory |
rowspan="2" colspan="2" |
! colspan="2" | Labial ! rowspan="2" | Coronal ! rowspan="2" | Dorsal ! rowspan="2" | Glottal |
---|
plain |
colspan="2"|Nasal
| {{IPA link|m}} | {{IPA link|mʷ}} | {{IPA link|n}} | {{IPA link|ŋ}} | |
rowspan="2"| Plosive
| {{IPA link|p}} | ({{IPA link|pʷ}}) | {{IPA link|t}} | {{IPA link|k}} | |
prenasalized
| {{IPA link|ᵐb}} | {{IPA link|ᵐbʷ}} | {{IPA link|ⁿd}} | {{IPA link|ᵑɡ}} | |
colspan="2"|Fricative
| {{IPA link|v}} | {{IPA link|vʷ}} | {{IPA link|s̠}} | [{{IPA link|ɣ}}] | {{IPA link|h}} |
colspan="2"|Trill
|colspan=2|{{IPA link|ʙ|ᵐbʙ⁽ʷ⁾}} | {{IPA link|r|ⁿdʳ}} | | |
colspan="2"|Tap
| | | {{IPA link|ɾ}} | | |
colspan="2"|Approximant
| | {{IPA link|w}} | {{IPA link|l}} | {{IPA link|j}} | |
{{IPA|/s/}} is post-alveolar. The voiceless stops are lightly aspirated. Otherwise, the consonants have the values their IPA transcriptions suggest.
{{IPA|/h/}} does not occur at the beginning of a word. Labialized consonants are only found before {{IPA|/a e i/}}. There are some grammatical contexts and perhaps random situations when word-initial {{IPA|/k/}} and {{IPA|/t/}} are replaced by {{IPA|/ɡ/}} and {{IPA|/d/}}. {{IPA|/pʷ/}} is known from only a single word. Word-final {{IPA|/k/}} is lost when the word is suffixed or followed by a modifier.
The prenasalized trills may be described as {{IPA|/mʙ, nr/}}, with the quite audible stop analyzed as excrescent, or as {{IPA|/bʙ, dr/}}, with the representation common in the area of prenasalized voiced stops as simply voiced stops. {{IPA|/ᵐbʙ/}} is quite common in the language. It is generally rounded, {{IPA|[mbʙʷ]}}, and word-finally the trilled release is at least partially devoiced, {{IPA|[mbʙ̥ʷ]}}. It may occur in word-final position after any vowel, but in CV position the following vowel is overwhelmingly {{IPA|/u/}}, though other vowels do occur, e.g. {{IPA|/suᵐbʙʷat/}} 'coral'. It is generated grammatically when the 3sg-irrealis {{IPA|/b⁽ʷ⁾V/}} is prefixed to a verb root beginning with {{IPA|/v, vʷ, v/}}, as in {{IPA|/bʷe-vʷel/}} > {{IPA|/ᵐbʙʷel/}} 's/he will come'.
==Consonant allophones==
Prenasalization is maintained after oral consonants, e.g. {{IPA|[ⁿdirⁿdir]}} 'earthquake', but is lost after a nasal, e.g. {{IPA|[luᵑɡamɡem]}} 'bamboo roof pins'. Prenasalized stops are occasionally devoiced word finally, e.g. {{IPA|[aⁿdʳaᵐb ~ aⁿdʳaᵐp]}} 'mud'.
{{IPA|/p/}} occasionally has a trilled release when followed by {{IPA|/ur/}}: {{IPA|[pʰura ~ pʙ̥ura]}} 'spit'.
Nasals and liquids are syllabified in word-final CN, CL clusters and in medial CNC, CLC clusters: {{IPA|[ᵑɡitn̩tl̩]}} 'we (paucal inclusive)', {{IPA|[kopm̩tl̩]}} 'we (paucal exclusive)'.
{{IPA|/k/}} is {{IPA|[k]}} word-initially, word-finally, before another consonant, and between front vowels; it is also the more common allophone between front and non-front vowels. It is {{IPA|[ɣ]}} between identical non-front vowels, and this is the more common allophone between non-identical non-front vowels.
{{IPA|/v, vʷ/}} are generally {{IPA|[f, fʷ]}} word-initially.
Nouns and Noun Phrases
=Pronouns=
The use of pronouns in Avava refer to what person the subject is in, the number of speakers, and the inclusivity, as shown in the table below
class="wikitable" |
colspan="2" |
! singular ! dual ! paucal ! plural |
---|
rowspan="2" | 1st person
! {{small|exclusive}} | rowspan="2" | na | kopmdru | kopmtl | kopm |
{{small|inclusive}}
| gitdru | gitntl | git |
colspan="2" | 2nd person
| ong | kamdru | kamtl | kam |
colspan="2" | 3rd person
| e | ierdru | iertl | ier |
The paucal form of a word vs the plural form of the word is generally characterized by the number of subjects. The paucal pronouns include a small number, greater than two but less than ten. The paucal and plural forms also differ systemically as they differ in the suffixes -dur and -tl.
=Nominalization=
The Avava language utilizes the process of nominalization to create words from pre-existing ones. Verbal nominalization of words involve the addition of the suffix -ian.
{{interlinear|number=ex:
|ran → ran-ian
|'dawn' → 'dawning'|}}
{{interlinear|number=ex:
|sasar → sasar-ian
|'teach' → 'teaching'|}}
In some cases, the nominalized form of a reduplicated verb contains the unreduplicated root.
{{interlinear|number=ex:
|ngarnar → ngar-ian
|'breathe' → 'breath'|}}
Another pattern of nominalization involves the addition of the suffix -ian as well as the addition of the first vowel of the word to the beginning of the word to create a noun from a verb.
{{interlinear|number=ex:
|kan → a-kan-iar
|'eat' → 'food'|}}
{{interlinear|number=ex:
|per → e-per-ian
|'work' → 'job'|}}
=Place of origin=
The prefix, ma-, when added to the name of a place, refers to a person that is from that specified area.
{{interlinear|number=ex:
|Viar → Ma-Viar
|'Viar' → {'people of Viar'}|}}
=Possession=
Nouns in Avava can be divided into two categories: directly possessed nouns and indirectly possessed nouns.
==Directly possessed nouns==
The following generalizations can be given on the subject of these types of nouns:
- most external body parts
- many internal organs, though some do not fall under this category
- some bodily products (saliva), though many do not fall under this category
- many body parts and products associated with these animals
- some kin terms (son/daughter)
- many parts of trees and plants
References
- Crowley, Terry (2006). The Avava language of central Malakula (Vanuatu). Canberra: Research School of Pacific and Asian Studies, The Australian National University.
{{Reflist}}
External links
- PARADISEC open-access archive of [http://catalog.paradisec.org.au/collections/search?language_id=6022 Avava language recordings]
{{Languages of Vanuatu}}
{{Southern Oceanic languages}}
{{Austronesian languages}}
{{authority control}}