Avava language

{{short description|Austronesian language spoken in Vanuatu}}

{{nf|date=June 2019}}

{{Infobox language

|name=Avava

|altname=Katbol

|region=Central Malekula

|state=Vanuatu

|speakers=700

|date=2001

|ref=e18

|familycolor=Austronesian

|fam2=Malayo-Polynesian

|fam3=Oceanic

|fam4=Southern Oceanic

|fam5=North-Central Vanuatu

|fam6=Central Vanuatu

|fam7=Malakula

|fam8=Malakula Interior

|iso3=tmb

|glotto=katb1237

|glottorefname=Avava

|map = Lang Status 99-NE.svg

|mapcaption = {{center|{{small|Avava is not endangered according to the classification system of the UNESCO Atlas of the World's Languages in Danger}}}}

}}

Avava (Navava), also known as Katbol, Tembimbe-Katbol, or Bangsa’ is an Oceanic language of central Malekula, Vanuatu. It has nasalized fricatives and a bilabial trill.

The four Avava-speaking villages speak or spoke, distinct dialects. Timbembe and Nevaar (Nɨviar) are still spoken. The Nivat (Nevat) and Bangasa (Umbrul) dialects are extinct. Bangasa/Bangsa', or more correctly Bangasak, was known as Numbuwul by its neighbors to the north; the endonym is Umbbuul {{IPA|tmb|(u)ᵐʙuːl|}}.

Names

The alternate names for Avava are Bangsa’, Katbol, Mallicolo, Navava, Taremp, Tembimbe-Katbol and Tisvel.{{Cite web |title=Glottolog 5.1 - Avava |url=https://glottolog.org/resource/languoid/id/katb1237 |access-date=2025-02-13 |website=glottolog.org}}

Phonology

When the final syllable is light (CV), stress tends to be penultimate. When the final syllable is heavy (CVC, CVV, CVː), stress tends to be final.

=Vowels=

There are a total of eight vowel quantities in Avava: five short vowels and three long vowels. The five short Avava vowel qualities, {{IPA|/a e i o u/}}. {{IPA|/u/}} is pronounced {{IPA|[ʉ]}} between a bilabial trill and an alveolar and, in final syllables, between a bilabial trill and {{IPA|/k/}}. About 2% of vowels are long. Long {{IPA|/eː/}} is not attested, and long {{IPA|/oː/}} is marginal. This is a pattern shared with Naman. At the end of a prosodic unit – in citation form, utterance-finally and when speaking slowly – word-final vowels other than {{IPA|/i/}} tend to be replaced with "diphthongs" {{IPA|/Vi/}}. Word-initial vowels present in citation form tend to be lost when the word is linked to others, e.g. when the subject of a verb or possessed by a pronoun. This is the reason for the alternative form of the name of the language, vava.

:

class="wikitable" style="text-align:center"

!

! front

! back

high

| {{IPA link|i}}

| {{IPA link|u}}

mid

| {{IPA link|e}}

| {{IPA link|o}}

low

| colspan="2"| {{IPA link|a}}

A notable variant of the same phoneme shown with short vowels is when /u/ undergoes centralisation to [ʉ] in two different settings: in closed syllables between a bilabial trill and a following alveolar consonant, and {{clarify|text=in non-final syllables between a bilabial trill and alveolar consonant|date=October 2019}}.

The three long vowels in Avava are /i:/, /u:/, and /a:/. Though there is evidence for the long /o:/, the vowel is only shown in three words throughout the entire lexicon of Avava.

=Consonants=

:

class="wikitable" style="text-align:center"

|+ Avava consonant inventory

rowspan="2" colspan="2" |

! colspan="2" | Labial

! rowspan="2" | Coronal

! rowspan="2" | Dorsal

! rowspan="2" | Glottal

plain

! labialized

colspan="2"|Nasal

| {{IPA link|m}}

| {{IPA link|mʷ}}

| {{IPA link|n}}

| {{IPA link|ŋ}}

|

rowspan="2"| Plosive

! voiceless

| {{IPA link|p}}

| ({{IPA link|pʷ}})

| {{IPA link|t}}

| {{IPA link|k}}

|

prenasalized

| {{IPA link|ᵐb}}

| {{IPA link|ᵐbʷ}}

| {{IPA link|ⁿd}}

| {{IPA link|ᵑɡ}}

|

colspan="2"|Fricative

| {{IPA link|v}}

| {{IPA link|vʷ}}

| {{IPA link|s̠}}

| [{{IPA link|ɣ}}]

| {{IPA link|h}}

colspan="2"|Trill

|colspan=2|{{IPA link|ʙ|ᵐbʙ⁽ʷ⁾}}

| {{IPA link|r|ⁿdʳ}}

|

|

colspan="2"|Tap

|

|

| {{IPA link|ɾ}}

|

|

colspan="2"|Approximant

|

| {{IPA link|w}}

| {{IPA link|l}}

| {{IPA link|j}}

|

{{IPA|/s/}} is post-alveolar. The voiceless stops are lightly aspirated. Otherwise, the consonants have the values their IPA transcriptions suggest.

{{IPA|/h/}} does not occur at the beginning of a word. Labialized consonants are only found before {{IPA|/a e i/}}. There are some grammatical contexts and perhaps random situations when word-initial {{IPA|/k/}} and {{IPA|/t/}} are replaced by {{IPA|/ɡ/}} and {{IPA|/d/}}. {{IPA|/pʷ/}} is known from only a single word. Word-final {{IPA|/k/}} is lost when the word is suffixed or followed by a modifier.

The prenasalized trills may be described as {{IPA|/mʙ, nr/}}, with the quite audible stop analyzed as excrescent, or as {{IPA|/bʙ, dr/}}, with the representation common in the area of prenasalized voiced stops as simply voiced stops. {{IPA|/ᵐbʙ/}} is quite common in the language. It is generally rounded, {{IPA|[mbʙʷ]}}, and word-finally the trilled release is at least partially devoiced, {{IPA|[mbʙ̥ʷ]}}. It may occur in word-final position after any vowel, but in CV position the following vowel is overwhelmingly {{IPA|/u/}}, though other vowels do occur, e.g. {{IPA|/suᵐbʙʷat/}} 'coral'. It is generated grammatically when the 3sg-irrealis {{IPA|/b⁽ʷ⁾V/}} is prefixed to a verb root beginning with {{IPA|/v, vʷ, v/}}, as in {{IPA|/bʷe-vʷel/}} > {{IPA|/ᵐbʙʷel/}} 's/he will come'.

==Consonant allophones==

Prenasalization is maintained after oral consonants, e.g. {{IPA|[ⁿdirⁿdir]}} 'earthquake', but is lost after a nasal, e.g. {{IPA|[luᵑɡamɡem]}} 'bamboo roof pins'. Prenasalized stops are occasionally devoiced word finally, e.g. {{IPA|[aⁿdʳaᵐb ~ aⁿdʳaᵐp]}} 'mud'.

{{IPA|/p/}} occasionally has a trilled release when followed by {{IPA|/ur/}}: {{IPA|[pʰura ~ pʙ̥ura]}} 'spit'.

Nasals and liquids are syllabified in word-final CN, CL clusters and in medial CNC, CLC clusters: {{IPA|[ᵑɡitn̩tl̩]}} 'we (paucal inclusive)', {{IPA|[kopm̩tl̩]}} 'we (paucal exclusive)'.

{{IPA|/k/}} is {{IPA|[k]}} word-initially, word-finally, before another consonant, and between front vowels; it is also the more common allophone between front and non-front vowels. It is {{IPA|[ɣ]}} between identical non-front vowels, and this is the more common allophone between non-identical non-front vowels.

{{IPA|/v, vʷ/}} are generally {{IPA|[f, fʷ]}} word-initially.

Nouns and Noun Phrases

=Pronouns=

The use of pronouns in Avava refer to what person the subject is in, the number of speakers, and the inclusivity, as shown in the table below

class="wikitable"
colspan="2" |

! singular

! dual

! paucal

! plural

rowspan="2" | 1st person

! {{small|exclusive}}

| rowspan="2" | na

| kopmdru

| kopmtl

| kopm

{{small|inclusive}}

| gitdru

| gitntl

| git

colspan="2" | 2nd person

| ong

| kamdru

| kamtl

| kam

colspan="2" | 3rd person

| e

| ierdru

| iertl

| ier

The paucal form of a word vs the plural form of the word is generally characterized by the number of subjects. The paucal pronouns include a small number, greater than two but less than ten. The paucal and plural forms also differ systemically as they differ in the suffixes -dur and -tl.

=Nominalization=

The Avava language utilizes the process of nominalization to create words from pre-existing ones. Verbal nominalization of words involve the addition of the suffix -ian.

{{interlinear|number=ex:

|ran → ran-ian

|'dawn' → 'dawning'|}}

{{interlinear|number=ex:

|sasar → sasar-ian

|'teach' → 'teaching'|}}

In some cases, the nominalized form of a reduplicated verb contains the unreduplicated root.

{{interlinear|number=ex:

|ngarnar → ngar-ian

|'breathe' → 'breath'|}}

Another pattern of nominalization involves the addition of the suffix -ian as well as the addition of the first vowel of the word to the beginning of the word to create a noun from a verb.

{{interlinear|number=ex:

|kan → a-kan-iar

|'eat' → 'food'|}}

{{interlinear|number=ex:

|per → e-per-ian

|'work' → 'job'|}}

=Place of origin=

The prefix, ma-, when added to the name of a place, refers to a person that is from that specified area.

{{interlinear|number=ex:

|Viar → Ma-Viar

|'Viar' → {'people of Viar'}|}}

=Possession=

Nouns in Avava can be divided into two categories: directly possessed nouns and indirectly possessed nouns.

==Directly possessed nouns==

The following generalizations can be given on the subject of these types of nouns:

  • most external body parts
  • many internal organs, though some do not fall under this category
  • some bodily products (saliva), though many do not fall under this category
  • many body parts and products associated with these animals
  • some kin terms (son/daughter)
  • many parts of trees and plants

References

  • Crowley, Terry (2006). The Avava language of central Malakula (Vanuatu). Canberra: Research School of Pacific and Asian Studies, The Australian National University.

{{Reflist}}